Neuropsychology Review

, Volume 22, Issue 4, pp 384–401 | Cite as

Language Development and Assessment in the Preschool Period

Review

Abstract

Most young children make significant progress in learning language during the first 4 years of life. Delays or differences in patterns of language acquisition are sensitive indicators of developmental problems. The dynamic, complex nature of language and the variability in the timing of its acquisition poses a number of challenges for the assessment of young children. This paper summarises the key developmental milestones of language development in the preschool years, providing a backdrop for understanding difficulties with language learning. Children with specific language impairment (SLI) are characterised illustrating the types of language difficulties they exhibit. Genetic evidence for language impairment suggests complex interactions among multiple genes of small effect. There are few consistent neurobiological abnormalities and currently there is no identified neurobiological signature for language difficulties. The assessment of young children’s language skills thus focuses on the evaluation of their performances in comparison to typically developing peers. Assessment of language abilities in preschool children should involve an evaluation of both expressive and receptive skills and should include an evaluation of more than one dimension of language. The use of a single measure of a language component, such as vocabulary, is considered inadequate for determining whether preschool children have typical language or language impairment. Available evidence supports the inclusion of measures of phonological short-term memory in the assessment of the language abilities of preschool children. Further study of genetic, neurobiological and early behavioural correlates of language impairments in preschool children is needed.

Keywords

Language development Language assessment Preschool children Specific Language Impairment (SLI) Phonological short term memory Psychometric instruments 

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Revised 4th Edition). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  2. Anisfeld, M., Rosenberg, E. S., Hoberman, M. J., & Gasparini, D. (1998). Lexical acceleration coincides with the onset of combinatorial speech. First Language, 18, 165–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006a). Nonword repetition: a comparison of tests. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 970–983.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006b). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41, 675–693.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Atance, C. M., & O’Neill, D. K. (2005). Preschoolers’ talk about future situations. First Language, 25, 5–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: an overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189–208.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Meldrum, D., et al. (2006). Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: the special needs and autism project (SNAP). Lancet, 368, 210–215.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Barre, N., Morgan, A., Doyle, L. W., & Anderson, P. J. (2011). Language abilities in children who were very preterm and/or very low birth weight: a meta-analysis. The Journal of Pediatrics, 158, 766–774.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bassano, D., Laaha, S., Maillochon, I., & Dressler, W. U. (2004). Early acquisition of verb grammar and lexical development: evidence from periphrastic constructions in French and Austrian German. First Language, 24, 33–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bates, E., Dale, P. S., & Thal, D. (1995). Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 96–151). Cambridge: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  12. Bates, E., & Goodman, J. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon. Evidence from acquisition, aphasia, and real-time processing. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12, 507–584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Behne, T., Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Twelve-month-olds’ comprehension and production of pointing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02043.x.
  14. Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language, 6, 183–200.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Bernhardt, B. M., Kemp, N., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Early word-object associations and later language development. First Language, 27, 315–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Cox, R. W., Rao, S. M., & Prieto, T. (1997). Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 353–362.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Uncommon understanding: development and disorders of language comprehension in children. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  18. Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC): a method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 879–893.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Bishop, D. V. M. (2002). The role of genes in the etiology of specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorder, 35, 311–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Bishop, D. V. M. (2006). Developmental cognitive genetics: how psychology can inform genetics and vice versa. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1153–1168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Bishop, D. V. M., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 1027–1050.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Bishop, D. V. M., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Language-impaired 4-year-olds: distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 156–173.Google Scholar
  23. Bishop, D. V. M., Holt, G., Line, E., McDonald, D., McDonald, S., & Watt, H. (2012). Parental phonological memory contributes to prediction of outcome of late talkers from 20 months to 4 years: a longitudinal study of precursors of specific language impairment. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. doi:10.1186/1866-1955-4-3.
  24. Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2002). Exploring the borderlands of autistic disorder and specific language impairment: a study using standardised diagnostic instruments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 917–930.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 37, 391–403.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Blank, M., Rose, S. A., & Berlin, L. J. (2003). Preschool language assessment instrument-second edition (PLAI-2). Austin: ProEd.Google Scholar
  27. Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C., & Haynes, O. M. (2004). Specific and general language performance across early childhood: stability and gender considerations. First Language, 24, 267–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2001). Non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36, 421–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Autism, primary pragmatic difficulties and specific language impairment: can we distinguish them using psycholinguistic markers? Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45, 515–524.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Braine, M. D. S. (1976). Children’s first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 41, 1–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Broca, P. (1861). Remarques sur le siege de la faculté du langage articulé; suivies d’une observation d’aphamie. Bulletins de la Société Anatomique de Paris, 6, 330–357.Google Scholar
  32. Bzoch, K. R., League, R., & Brown, V. L. (2003). Receptive-expressive emergent language test third edition (REEL-3). San Antonio: Pearson.Google Scholar
  33. Camaioni, L., Castelli, M. C., Longobardi, E., & Volterra, V. (1991). A parent report instrument for early language assessment. First Language, 11, 345–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Campbell, T., Dollaghan, C., Needleman, H., & Janosky, J. (1997). Reducing bias in language assessment: processing-dependent measures. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 519–525.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Catani, M., Jones, D. K., & Ffytche, D. H. (2005). Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Annals of Neurology, 57, 5–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Chapman, R. S. (1978). Comprehension strategies in children. In J. F. Kavanaugh & W. Strange (Eds.), Speech and language in the laboratory, school, and clinic (pp. 308–327). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  37. Chapman, S. B., Max, J. E., Gamino, J. F., McGlothlin, J. H., & Cliff, S. N. (2003). Discourse plasticity in children after stroke: age at injury and lesion effects. Pediatric Neurology, 29, 34–41.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Cheng, L. (1990). The identification of communication disorders in Asian-Pacific students. Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders, 13, 113–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Cheng, L. (1997). Diversity: challenges and implications for assessment. Journal of Children’s Communication Development, 19, 55–62.Google Scholar
  40. Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2007). The preschool repetition test: an evaluation of performance in typically developing and clinically referred children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 429–776.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2008). Early phonological and sociocognitive skills as predictors of later language and social communication outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 635–645.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Clark, M., & Plante, E. (1998). Morphology of the inferior frontal gyrus in developmentally language-disordered adults. Brain and Language, 61, 288–303.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Processing and linguistic markers in young children with specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1029–1037.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N. (1999a). Classification of children with specific language impairment: longitudinal considerations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1195–1204.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N. (1999b). Characteristics of children attending language units in England: a national study of 7-year-olds. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 34, 359–366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741–748.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Conti-Ramsden, G., & Durkin, K. (2007). Phonological short-term memory, language and literacy: developmental relationships in early adolescence in young people with SLI. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 147–156.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Conti-Ramsden, G., & Durkin, K. (2011). Specific language impairment. In D. Skuse, H. Bruce, L. Dowdney, & D. Mrazek (Eds.), Child psychology and psychiatry: frameworks for practice (pp. 180–186). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Conti-Ramsden, G., Durkin, K., Simkin, Z., & Knox, E. (2009). Specific language impairment and school outcomes. I: identifying and explaining variability at the end of compulsory education. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44, 15–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Conti-Ramsden, G., & Jones, M. (1997). Verb use in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1298–1313.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Conti-Ramsden, G., St. Clair, M., Pickles, A., & Durkin, K. (2012). Developmental trajectories of verbal and nonverbal skills in individuals with a history of SLI: from childhood to adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2012/10-0182)
  52. Crutchley, A., Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (1997). Bilingualism and specific language impairment in children attending language units. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 32, 267–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Culp, R. E., Watkins, R. V., Lawrence, H., Letts, D., Kelly, D. J., & Rice, M. L. (1991). Maltreated children’s language and speech development: abused, neglected, and abused and neglected. First Language, 11, 377–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Dawson, J., Eyer, J. A., & Fonkalsrud, J. (2005). Structured photographic expressive language test—preschool (2nd ed.). DeKalb: Janelle Publications.Google Scholar
  55. De Guibert, C., Maumet, C., Jannin, P., Ferré, J.-C., Tréguier, C., Barillot, C., et al. (2011). Abnormal functional lateralization and activity of language brain areas in typical specific language impairment (developmental dysphasia). Brain, 134, 3044–3058.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. De Lamo White, C., & Jin, L. (2011). Evaluation of speech and language assessment approaches with bilingual children. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 46, 613–627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. De Smet, H. J., Baillieux, H., De Deyn, P. P., Marien, P., & Paquier, P. (2007). The cerebellum and language: the story so far. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 59, 165–170.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., Boivin, M., & Plomin, R. (2003). Genetic evidence for directional effects of early lexical and grammatical development. Child Development, 74, 394–412.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Dockrell, J. E. (2001). Assessing language skills in preschool children. Child Psychology & Psychiatry Review, 6, 74–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test - IV (PPVT-IV). San Antonio: Pearson.Google Scholar
  61. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2009). British picture vocabulary scale: third edition (BPVS-III). London: GL Assessment Limited.Google Scholar
  62. Durkin, K. (1987). Minds and language: social cognition, social conflict and the acquisition of language. Mind and Language, 2, 105–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Durkin, K. (1995). Developmental social psychology. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
  64. Durkin, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2007). Language, social behavior, and the quality of friendships in adolescents with and without a history of specific language impairment. Child Development, 78, 1441–1457.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Edwards, S., Letts, C., & Sinka, I. (2011). New Reynell developmental language scales. London: GL Assessment Limited.Google Scholar
  66. Eigsti, I. M., & Cicchetti, D. (2004). The impact of child maltreatment on expressive syntax at 60 months. Developmental Science, 7, 88–102.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Elliot, C. D., & Smith, P. (2011). British ability scales: third edition (BAS3). London: GL Assessment Limited.Google Scholar
  68. Eriksson, M., & Berglund, E. (1999). Swedish early communicative development inventories: words and gestures. First Language, 19, 55–90.Google Scholar
  69. Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pereira, M. P., Wehberg, S. Marjanovič-Umek, L., et al. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: evidence from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02042.x.
  70. Falcaro, M., Pickles, A., Newbury, D. F., Addis, L., Banfield, E., Fisher, S. E., Monaco, A. P., Simkin, Z., Conti-Ramsden, G., & The SLI Consortium. (2008). Genetic and phenotypic effects of phonological short-term memory and grammatical morphology in specific language impairment. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 7, 393–402.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Feldman, H. M., Holland, A. L., Kemp, S. S., & Janosky, J. E. (1992). Language development after unilateral brain injury. Brain and Language, 42, 89–102.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 1–173.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories (CDIs). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc.Google Scholar
  74. Fernald, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2011). Causes and consequences of variability in early language learning. In I. Arnon & E. V. Clark (Eds.), Experience, variation, and generalization: learning a first language (Trends in Language Acquisition Research, vol. 7) (pp. 179–202). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  75. Fischel, J., Whitehurst, G., Caulfield, M., & De Baryshe, B. (1989). Language growth in children with expressive language delay. Paediatrics, 83, 218–227.Google Scholar
  76. Friederici, A. D., Oberecker, R., & Brauer, J. (2012). Neurophysiological preconditions of syntax acquisition. Psychological Research, 76, 204–211.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Fusaro, M., Harris, P. L., & Pan, B. A. (2012). Head nodding and head shaking gestures in children’s early communication. First Language. doi:10.1177/0142723711419326.
  78. Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of STM in the development of vocabulary in children: a longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language-disordered children: is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1996). The children’s test of nonword repetition. London: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  81. Gauger, L. M., Lombardino, L. J., & Leonard, C. M. (1997). Brain morphology in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1272–1284.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Glascoe, F. P. (1997). Parents’ concerns about children’s development: prescreening technique or screening test? Pediatrics, 99, 522–528.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Gogate, L. K., & Hollich, G. (2010). Invariance detection within an interactive system: a perceptual gateway to language development. Psychological Review, 117, 496–516.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language, 17, 171–184.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Gray, S., Plante, E., & Vance, R. (1999). The diagnostic accuracy of four vocabulary tests administered to preschool-age children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 196–206.Google Scholar
  86. Griffin, T. M., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. P. (2004). Oral discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First Language, 24, 123–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Hedrick, D. L., Prather, E. M., & Tobin, A. R. (1984). Sequenced inventory of communication development, revised (SICD-R). Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
  88. Hoff, E. (2009). Language development (4th ed.). Belmont: Wadsworth/Cengage LearningGoogle Scholar
  89. Hoff, E. (2010). Context effects on young children’s language use: the influence of conversational setting and partner. First Language, 30, 461–472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Holland, S. K., Vannest, J., Mecoli, M., Jacola, L. M., Tillema, J.-M., Karunanayaka, P. R., et al. (2007). Functional MRI of language lateralization during development in children. International Journal of Audiology, 46, 533–551.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Hresko, W. P., Reid, D. K., & Hammill, D. D. (1999). The test of early language development-3 (TELD-3). Austin: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
  92. Jackson, T., & Plante, E. (1996). Gyral morphology in the posterior sylvian region in families affected by developmental language disorder. Neuropyschology Review, 6, 81–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Marchman, V., Bates, E., & Gutierrez-Clellen, V. (1993). Early lexical development in Spanish-speaking infants and toddlers. Journal of Child Language, 20, 523–549.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 389–398.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Kern, S. (2007a). Influence of preterm birth on early lexical and grammatical development. First Language, 27, 159–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Kern, S. (2007b). Lexicon development in French-speaking infants. First Language, 27, 227–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Kirjavainen, M., Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2009). I want hold Postman Pat’: an investigation into the acquisition of infinitival marker ‘to’. First Language, 29, 313–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Klee, T., Carson, D., Gavin, W., Hall, L., Kent, A., & Rece, S. (1998). Concurrent and predictive validity of an early language screening programme. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 26, 33–38.Google Scholar
  99. Krentz, U. C., & Corina, D. P. (2008). Preference for language in early infancy: the human language bias is not speech specific. Developmental Science, 11, 1–9.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Evolution, nativism and learning the development of language and speech. In M. Gopnik (Ed.), The inheritance and innateness of grammars (pp. 7–44). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  101. Laakso, M., & Soininen, M. (2010). Mother-initiated repair sequences in interactions of 3-year-old children. First Language, 30, 329–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Law, J., Boyle, J., Harris, F., Harkness, A., & Nye, C. (1998). Screening for speech and language delay: a systematic review of the literature. Health Technology Assessment, 2, 1–184.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Law, J., Garrett, Z., & Nye, C. (2003). The efficacy of treatment for children with developmental speech and language delay/disorder: a meta analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 924–943.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Law, J., Garrett, Z., & Nye, C. (2010). Speech and language therapy interventions for children with primary speech and language delay and disorder (Review). The Cochrane Library, issue 5, 1–77. The Cochrane Collaboration. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
  105. Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  106. Leonard, C. M., Eckert, M. A., & Kuldau, J. M. (2006). Exploiting human anatomical variability as a link between genome and cognome. Genes, Brain & Behaviour, 5, 64–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Locke, J. L. (1989). Babbling and early speech: continuity and individual differences. First Language, 9, 191–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Lovas, G. S. (2011). Gender and patterns of language development in mother-toddler and father-toddler dyads. First Language, 31, 83–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Lum, J. A. G., Conti-Ramsden, G., Page, D., & Ullman, M. T. (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.001.
  110. Majorano, M., & D’Odorico, L. (2011). The transition into ambient language: a longitudinal study of babbling and first word production of Italian children. First Language, 31, 47–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Martin, N. A., & Brownell, R. (2010a). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test Fourth Edition (EOWPVT-4). Novato: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
  112. Martin, N. A., & Brownell, R. (2010b). Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test Fourth Edition (ROWPVT-4). Novato: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
  113. Masur, E. F., Flynn, V., & Lloyd, C. A. (2012). Infants‘ behaviors as antecedents and consequents of mothers’ responsive and directive utterances. First Language. Google Scholar
  114. McKinley, N., & Larson, V. (1989). Students who can’t communicate: speech-language service at the secondary level. Curriculum Report, 19, 1–8.Google Scholar
  115. McMurray, B., & Aslin, R. N. (2005). Infants are sensitive to within category variation in speech perception. Cognition, 95, B15–B26.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Miller, J. F. (1981). Assessing language production in children. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  117. Miller, C. A., Leonard, L. B., & Finneran, D. (2008). Grammaticality judgements in adolescents with and without language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43, 346–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  118. Misyak, J. B., & Christiansen, M. H. (2011). Genetic variation and individual differences in language. In I. Arnon & E. V. Clark (Eds.), Experience, variation, and generalization: learning a first language (Trends in Language Acquisition Research, vol. 7) (pp. 223–238). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  119. Moyle, M. J., Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J. L., & Lindstrom, M. J. (2007). Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammatical development in typical and late talking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 508–528.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Newbury, D. F., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Paracchini, S., Buckingham, L., Clark, A., et al. (2009). CMIP and ATP2C2 modulate phonological short-term memory in language impairment. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 85, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. Nippold, M. A., & Schwarz, I. E. (2002). Do children recover from specific language impairment? Advances in Speech-Language Pathology, 1, 41–49.Google Scholar
  122. O’Toole, C., & Fletcher, P. (2010). Validity of a parent report for Irish-speaking toddlers. First Language, 30, 199–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. O’Neill, D. K., Main, R. M., & Ziemski, R. A. (2009). ‘I like Barney’: preschoolers’ spontaneous conversational initiations with peers. First Language, 29, 401–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. Papousek, M., & Papousek, H. (1989). Forms and functions of vocal matching in interactions between mothers and their precanonical infants. First Language, 9, 137–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Paradis, J. (2005). Grammatical morphology in children learning English as a second language: implications for similarities with specific language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 172–187.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  126. Paul, R. (1996). Clinical implications of the natural history of slow expressive language development. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 5, 5–21.Google Scholar
  127. Paul, R. (2006). Language disorders from infancy through adolescence: assessment and intervention (3rd ed.). St. Louis: Mosby-Year Book.Google Scholar
  128. Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Conti-Ramsden, G., Falcaro, M., Simkin, Z., Charman, T., et al. (2009). Loss of language in early development of autism and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 843–852.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  129. Powell, H. W., Parker, G. J., Alexander, D. C., Symms, M. R., Boulby, P. A., Wheeler-Kingshott, C. A., et al. (2006). Hemispheric asymmetries in language-related pathways: a combined functional MRI and tractography study. NeuroImage, 32, 388–399.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  130. Rapin, I. (1996). Developmental language disorders: a clinical update. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 643–656.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Reilly, J. S., Bates, E. A., & Marchman, V. A. (1998). Narrative discourse in children with early focal brain injury. Brain and Language, 61, 335–375.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  132. Reilly, S., Bavin, E. L., Bretherton, L., Conway, L., Eadie, P., Cini, E., et al. (2009). The Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS): a prospective, longitudinal study of communication skills and expressive vocabulary development at 8, 12 and 24 months. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 344–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  133. Reilly, S., Wake, M., Ukoumunne, O. C., Bavin, E., Prior, M., Cini, E., et al. (2010). Predicting language outcomes at 4 years of age: findings from early language Victoria study. Pediatrics, 126, e1530–e1537.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  134. Renfrew, C. (1998). Renfrew language scales. Chesterfield: Winslow Press.Google Scholar
  135. Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (2001). Rice/Wexler test of early grammar impairment. SanAntonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  136. Robinson, R. J. (1991). Causes and associations of severe and persistent specific speech and language disorders in children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 33, 943–962.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  137. Roulstone, S., Loader, S., Northstone, K., Beveridge, M., & the ALSPAC team. (2002). The speech and language of children age 25 months: descriptive data from the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Early Childhood Development and Care, 172, 259–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  138. Rutter, D. R., & Durkin, K. (1987). Turn-taking in mother–infant interaction: an examination of vocalizations and gaze. Developmental Psychology, 25, 54–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  139. Saaristo-Helin, K., Kunnari, S., & Savinainen-Makkonen, T. (2011). Phonological development in children learning Finnish: a review. First Language, 31, 342–363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  140. Saudino, K. J., Dale, P. S., Oliver, B., Petrill, S. A., Richardson, V., Rutter, M., et al. (1998). The validity of parent-based assessment of the cognitive abilities of 2-year-olds. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 349–363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  141. Schipke, C. S., & Kauschke, C. (2011). Early word formation in German language acquisition: a study on word formation growth during the second and third years. First Language, 31, 67–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  142. Seeff-Gabriel, B., Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2008). Early repetition battery. London: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
  143. Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (2006). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals - preschool 2 UK edition (CELF-Preschool2 UK). London: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
  144. Shatz, M. (1978). On the development of communicative understandings: an early strategy for interpreting and responding to messages. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 271–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  145. Short-Meyerson, K. J. (2010). Preschoolers’ establishment of mutual knowledge during scripted interactions. First Language, 30, 219–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  146. Silva, P. A. (1987). Epidemiology, longitudinal course, and some associated factors: An update. In W. Yule & M. Rutter (Eds.), Language development and disorders (pp. 1–15). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
  147. Simkin, Z., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006). Evidence of reading difficulties in subgroups of children with specific language impairment. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 22, 315–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  148. Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland adaptive behavior scales 2nd edition (Vineland-II). Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  149. St Clair, M. C., Durkin, K., Conti-Ramsden, G., & Pickles, A. (2010). Growth of reading skills in children with a history of specific language impairment: the role of autistic symptomatology and language-related abilities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 109–132.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  150. St Clair, M. C., Pickles, A., Durkin, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2011). A longitudinal study of behavioural, emotional and social difficulties in individuals with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Communication Disorders, 44, 186–199.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  151. Stefanatos, G. A., & Baron, I. S. (2011). The ontogenesis of language impairment in autism: a neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychological Review, 21, 252–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  152. Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., & Lehtonen, L. (2008). Early lexical development of Finnish children – a longitudinal study. First Language, 28, 259–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  153. Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B., & Kaplan, C. (1998). Language impaired preschoolers: a follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 41, 407–418.Google Scholar
  154. Szagun, G., Steinbrink, C., Franik, M., & Stumper, B. (2006). Development of vocabulary and grammar in young German-speaking children assessed with a German language development inventory. First Language, 26, 259–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  155. Thal, D., & Katch, J. (1996). Predicaments in early identification of specific language impairment: does the early bird always catch the worm? In K. Cole, P. Dale, & D. Thal (Eds.), Assessment of communication and language (pp. 1–28). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
  156. Thal, D., Tobias, S., & Morrison, D. (1991). Language and gesture in late talkers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 604–612.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  157. Thomas, M. S. C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Are developmental disorders like cases of adult brain damage? Implications from connectionist modelling. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 727–788.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  158. Thordardottir, E. T., & Ellis Weismer, S. (1996). Language assessment via parent report: development of a screening instrument for Icelandic children. First Language, 16, 265–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  159. Thordardottir, E. T., Ellis Weismer, S., & Evans, J. L. (2002). Continuity in lexical and morphological development in Icelandic and English-speaking 2-year-olds. First Language, 22, 3–28.Google Scholar
  160. Tincoff, R., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1999). Some beginnings of word comprehension in 6-month-olds. Psychological Science, 10, 172–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  161. Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  162. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78, 705–722.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  163. Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  164. Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., & Zhang, X. (1996). A system for the diagnosis of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39, 1284–1294.Google Scholar
  165. Tyack, D., & Ingram, D. (1977). Children’s production and comprehension of questions. Journal of Child Language, 4, 211–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  166. Veneziano, E., & Parisse, C. (2010). The acquisition of early verbs in French: assessing the role of conversation and of child-directed input. First Language, 30, 287–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  167. Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech (N. Minick, Trans.). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  168. Wake, M., Tobin, S., Girolametto, L., Okoumunne, O. C., Gold, L., Levickis, P., et al. (2011). Outcomes of population based language promotion for slow to talk toddlers at ages 2 and 3 years: let’s learn language cluster randomised control trial. British Medical Journal, 343, d4741.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  169. Watkins, K. E., Vargha-Khadem, F., Ashburner, J., Passingham, R. E., Connelly, A., Friston, K. J., et al. (2002). MRI analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: structural brain abnormalities. Brain, 125, 465–478.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  170. Webster, R. I., & Shevell, M. I. (2004). Neurobiology of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Neurology, 19, 471–481.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  171. Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence – third edition UK (WPPSI-III UK). Oxford: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  172. Wehberg, S., Vach, W., Bleses, D., Thomsen, P., Madsen, T. O., & Basbøll, H. (2007). Danish children’s first words – analysing longitudinal data based on monthly CDI parental reports. First Language, 27, 361–383.Google Scholar
  173. Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische symptomenkomplex. Breslau: M. Cohn und Weigert.Google Scholar
  174. Wiig, E. H., Secord, W., & Semel, E. (1992). CELF-preschool: clinical evaluation of language fundamentals—preschool version. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  175. Wiig, E. H., Secord, W. A., & Semel, E. (2006). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals-preschool: Australian standardised edition (2nd ed.). Sydney: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
  176. Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive vocabulary test, second edition (EVT-2). San Antonio: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
  177. World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 classification for mental and behavioural disorders: diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
  178. Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (1979). Preschool language scale. Columbus: Charles Merrill.Google Scholar
  179. Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2002). Preschool language scale, fourth edition (PLS-4): Australian language adapted. Australia: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  180. Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2008). Preschool language scales UK adaptation (PLS-4UK). London: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  181. Zubrick, S. R., Taylor, C. L., Rice, M. L., & Slegers, D. W. (2007). Late language emergence at 24 months: an epidemiological study of prevalence, predictors, and covariates. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1562–1592.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Human Communication and DeafnessThe University of ManchesterManchesterUK
  2. 2.School of Psychological Sciences and HealthUniversity of StrathclydeGlasgowUK

Personalised recommendations