Advertisement

Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology

, Volume 49, Issue 9, pp 1159–1168 | Cite as

Improvements in the Recognition of Sounds after Presentation during Sleep

  • Yu. V. UkraintsevaEmail author
  • K. M. Liaukovich
  • K. L. Sake
  • A. A. Polishchuk
  • O. V. Martynova
Article
  • 9 Downloads

The effect of targeted memory reactivation during sleep on auditory recognition remains unstudied. We report here our verification that the recognition of sounds which are difficult to discriminate can be improved by repeat presentation during daytime sleep following training. After two sequential sessions of recognizing one of two deviant sounds in the oddball paradigm, subjects of the study group were presented with one of the recognized deviants in sleep stage 3 (slow-wave sleep, SWS). The control group was presented with a novel deviant during sleep, which had not been presented for recognition. After sleep, the study group showed significant improvement in the recognition of sounds presented during SWS, which was accompanied by an increase in the amplitude and a decrease in the latency of the mismatch negativity (MMN) component of the event-related potential. The control group showed no significant differences in discriminating sounds or in MMN parameters after sleep. Our data provide evidence that targeted reactivation of sound memory during sleep promotes the neuroplastic rearrangements underlying learning sound recognition.

Keywords

memory reactivation slow-wave sleep auditory recognition mismatch negativity event-related potentials EEG 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Antony, J. W., Gobel, E. W., O’Hare, J. K., et al., “Cued memory reactivation during sleep influences skill learning,” Nat. Neurosci., 15, No. 8, 1114–1116 (2012).PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Batterink, L. J., Creery, J. D., and Paller, K. A., “Phase of spontaneous slow oscillations during sleep influences memory-related processing of auditory cues,” J. Neurosci., 36, No. 4, 1401–1409 (2016).PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Bekinschtein, T. A., Dehaene, S., Rohaut, B., et al., “Neural signature of the conscious processing of auditory regularities,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 106, No. 5, 1672–1677 (2009).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bergmann, T. O., Mölle, M., Diedrichs, J., et al., “Sleep spindle-related reactivation of category-specific cortical regions after learning facescene associations,” NeuroImage, 59, No. 3, 2733–2742 (2012).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bigelow, J. and Poremba, A., “Achilles’ ear? Inferior human short-term and recognition memory in the auditory modality,” PLoS One, 9, No. 2 (2014), journal.pone.0089914.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Carskadon, M. A. and Dement, W. C., “Normal human sleep: an overview,” in: Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, Kryge, M. H., Roth, T., and Dement, W. C. (eds.) Elsevier Saunders, St. Louis, Missouri (2011).Google Scholar
  7. Diekelmann, S., Büchel, C., Born, J., and Rasch, B., “Labile or stable: opposing consequences for memory when reactivated during waking and sleep,” Nat. Neurosci., 14, No. 3, 381–6 (2011).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Diekelmann, S., “Sleep for cognitive enhancement,” Front. Syst. Neurosci., 8, No. 46, 1–12 (2014).Google Scholar
  9. Diekelmann, S. and Born, J., “The memory function of sleep,” Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 11, No. 2, 114–126 (2010).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Dudai, Y., Karni, A., and Born, J., “The consolidation and transformation of memory,” Neuron, 88, No. 1, 20–32 (2015).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Ego-Stengel, V. and Wilson, M. A., “Disruption of ripple-associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat,” Hippocampus, 20, No. 1, 1–10 (2011).Google Scholar
  12. Forcato, C., Burgos, V. L., Argibay, P. F., et al., “Reconsolidation of declarative memory in humans,” Learn. Mem., 14, No. 4, 295–303 (2007).PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Gregg, M. K. and Snyder, J. S., “Enhanced sensory processing accompanies successful detection of change for real-world sounds,” Neuroimage, 62, 113–119 (2012).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Hars, B., Hennevin, E., and Pasques, P., “Improvement of learning by cueing during postlearning paradoxical sleep,” Behav. Brain Res., 18, 241–250 (1985).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Hilditch, C. J., Dorrian, J., and Banks, S., “A review of short naps and sleep inertia: do naps of 30 min or less really avoid sleep inertia and slow-wave sleep?” Sleep Med., 32, 176–190 (2017).Google Scholar
  16. Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., Hardt, O., and Nadel, L., “Reconsolidation of episodic memories: A subtle reminder triggers integration of new information,” Learn. Mem., 14, No. 1–2, 47–53 (2007).PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Ji, D. and Wilson, M. A., “Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep,” Nat. Neurosci., 10, No. 1, 100–107 (2007).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Kumar, S., Bonnici, H. M., Teki, S., et al., “Representations of specific acoustic patterns in the auditory cortex and hippocampus,” Proc. Roy. Soc. B. Biol. Sci., 281, No. 1791 (2014), doi/10.1098/rspb.2014.1000.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Lang, H. A., Nyrke, T., Ek, M., et al., “Pitch discrimination performance and auditory event-related potentials,” in: Psychophysiological Brain Research, Brunia, C. H. M. et al. (eds.), Tilburg University Press, Tilburg (1990), Vol. 1, pp. 294–298.Google Scholar
  20. Maingret, N., Girardeau, G., Todorova, R., et al., “Hippocampo-cortical coupling mediates memory consolidation during sleep,” Nat. Neurosci., 19, No. 7, 959–964 (2016).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Maret, S., Faraguna, U., Nelson, A. B., et al., “Sleep and waking modulate spine turnover in the adolescent mouse cortex,” Nat. Neurosci., 14, No. 11, 1418–1420 (2011).PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Miyamoto, D., Hirai, D., Fung, C. C. A., et al., “Top-down cortical input during NREM sleep consolidates perceptual memory,” Science, 352, No. 6291, 1315–1318 (2016).Google Scholar
  23. Näätänen, R., “Mismatch negativity (MMN) as an index of central auditory system plasticity,” Int. J. Audiol., 47, Supplement 2, 16–20 (2008).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Näätänen, R., Schroger, E., Karakas, S., et al., “Development of a memory trace for a complex sound in the human brain,” Neuroreport, 4, 503–506 (1993).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Nacher, V., Ledberg, A., Deco, G., and Romo, R., “Coherent delta-band oscillations between cortical areas correlate with decision making,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 110, No. 37, 15085–15090 (2013).Google Scholar
  26. Nader, K. and Hardt, O., “A single standard for memory: the case for reconsolidation,” Neuroscience, 10, 224–234 (2009).Google Scholar
  27. Pakarinen, S., Takegata, R., Rinne, T., et al., “Measurement of extensive auditory discrimination profiles using the mismatch negativity (MMN) of the auditory event-related potential (ERP),” Clin. Neurophysiol., 118, 177–185 (2007).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Peigneux, P., Laureys, S., Fuchs, S., et al., “Are spatial memories strengthened in the human hippocampus during slow wave sleep?” Neuron, 44, No. 3, 535–545 (2004).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Peters, J., Suchan, B., Köster, O., and Daum, I., “Domain-specific retrieval of source information in the medial temporal lobe,” Eur. J. Neurosci., 26, No. 5, 1333–1343 (2007).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Pourtois, G., De Pretto, M., Hauert, C.-A., and Vuilleumier, P., “Time course of brain activity during change blindness and change awareness: performance is predicted by neural events before change onset,” J. Cogn. Neurosci., 18, No. 12, 2108–2129 (2006).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Rasch, B. and Born, J., “About sleep’s role in memory,” Physiol. Rev., 93, No. 2, 681–766 (2013).PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Rasch, B., Buchel, C., Gais, S., and Born, J., “Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation,” Science, 315, No. 5817, 1426–1429 (2007).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Rechtschaffen, A. and Kales, A. A., Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects, Brain Information Service/Brain Research Institute, University of California, Los Angeles, CA (1968).Google Scholar
  34. Rudoy, J. D., Voss, J. L., Westerberg, C. E., and Paller, K. A., “Strengthening individual memories by reactivating them during sleep,” Science, 326, No. 5956, 1079 (2009).PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Sams, M., Paavilainen, P., Alho, K., and Näätänen, R., “Auditory frequency discrimination and event-related potentials,” EEG Clin. Neurophysiol./Evoked Potentials, 62, No. 6, 437–448 (1985).Google Scholar
  36. Schönauer, M., Geisler, T., and Gais, S., “Strengthening procedural memories by reactivation in sleep,” J. Cogn. Neurosci., 26, No. 1, 143–53 (2014).Google Scholar
  37. Schreiner, T., Goldi, M., and Rasch, B., “Cueing vocabulary during sleep increases theta activity during later recognition testing,” Psychophysiology, 52, No. 11, 1538–1543 (2015).Google Scholar
  38. Schreiner, T. and Rasch, B., “Boosting vocabulary learning by verbal cueing during sleep,” Cereb. Cortex, 25, No. 11, 4169–4179 (2015).Google Scholar
  39. Siapas, A. G. and Wilson, M. A., “Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep,” Neuron, 21, 1123–1128 (1998).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Skaggs, W. E. and McNaughton, B. L., “Replay of neuronal firing sequences in rat hippocampus during sleep following spatial experience,” Science, 271, No. 5257, 1870–1873 (1996).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Smith, C. and Weeden, K., “Post training REMs coincident auditory stimulation enhances memory in humans,” Psychiatr. J. Univ. Ott., 15, 85–90 (1990).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Squire, L., Schmolck, H., and Stark, S., “Impaired auditory recognition memory in amnesic patients with medial temporal lobe lesions,” Learn. Mem., 8, No. 5, 252–256 (2001).PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  43. Stickgold, R. and Walker, M. P., “Sleep-dependent memory triage: evolving generalization through selective processing,” Nat. Neurosci., 16, No. 2, 139–145 (2013).PubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. Timofeev, I. and Chauvette, S., “Sleep slow oscillation and plasticity,” Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 44, 116–126 (2017).Google Scholar
  45. Tononi, G. and Cirelli, C., “Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration,” Neuron, 81, No. 1, 12–34 (2014).PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Trotti, L. M., “Waking up is the hardest thing I do all day: Sleep inertia and sleep drunkenness,” Sleep Med. Rev., 35, 76–84 (2017).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Weinberger, N. M., “Specific long-term memory traces in primary auditory cortex,” Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 5, 279–290 (2004).PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  48. Wickens, T. D., Elementary Signal Detection Theory, OUP USA, ISBN 0-19-509250-3 (2001), Chapter 2, p. 20.Google Scholar
  49. Wilson, M. A. and McNaughton, B. L., “Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep,” Science, 256, 676–679 (1994).Google Scholar
  50. Yang, G., Lai, C. S., Cichon, J., et al., “Sleep promotes branch-specific formation of dendritic spines after learning,” Science, 344, 1173 (2014).PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Yu. V. Ukraintseva
    • 1
    Email author
  • K. M. Liaukovich
    • 1
    • 3
  • K. L. Sake
    • 3
  • A. A. Polishchuk
    • 1
    • 4
  • O. V. Martynova
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Institute of Higher Nervous Activity and NeurophysiologyRussian Academy of SciencesMoscowRussia
  2. 2.Neuroeconomics and Cognitive Research CenterNational Research University Higher School of EconomicsMoscowRussia
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyNational Research University Higher School of EconomicsMoscowRussia
  4. 4.Lomonosov Moscow State UniversityMoscowRussia

Personalised recommendations