Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 43, Issue 4, pp 662–680 | Cite as

Moral self-judgment is stronger for future than past actions

  • Hallgeir SjåstadEmail author
  • Roy F. Baumeister
Original Paper

Abstract

When, if ever, would a person want to be held responsible for his or her choices? Across four studies (N = 915), people favored more extreme rewards and punishments for their future than their past actions. This included thinking that they should receive more blame and punishment for future misdeeds than for past ones, and more credit and reward for future good deeds than for past ones. The tendency to moralize the future more than the past was mediated by anticipating (one’s own) emotional reactions and concern about one’s reputation, which was stronger in the future as well. The findings fit the pragmatic view that people moralize the future partly to guide their choices and actions, such as by increasing their motivation to restrain selfish impulses and build long-term cooperative relationships with others. People typically believe that the future is open and changeable, while the past is not. We conclude that the psychology of moral accountability has a strong future component.

Keywords

Morality Self-judgment Prospection Emotion Reputation 

Notes

Funding

This work was partly supported by the Norway-America Association (NORAM), and the Research Council of Norway through its Centres of Excellence Scheme, FAIR Project No 262675. Data and materials for all four studies are openly available online: https://www.osf.io/z95rm.

References

  1. Alicke, M. D. (2000). Culpable control and the psychology of blame. Psychological Bulletin, 126(4), 556–574.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Balcetis, E., & Cardenas, A. (2018).Visual experience in self and social judgment: How a biased majority claim a superior minority. Self and Identity.  https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2018.1466724.
  3. Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3(3), 193–209.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Suicide as escape from self. Psychological Review, 97(1), 90–113.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Escaping the self. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  6. Baumeister, R. F., Ainsworth, S. E., & Vohs, K. D. (2016a). Are groups more or less than the sum of their members? The moderating role of individual identification. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Baumeister, R. F., & Juola Exline, J. (1999). Virtue, personality, and social relations: Self-control as the moral muscle. Journal of Personality, 67(6), 1165–1194.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., DeWall, C. N., & Zhang, L. (2007). How emotion shapes behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(2), 167–203.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Oettingen, G. (2016b). Pragmatic prospection: How and why people think about the future. Review of General Psychology, 20(1), 3–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Caruso, E. M. (2010). When the future feels worse than the past: A temporal inconsistency in moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Caruso, E. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2008). A wrinkle in time: Asymmetric valuation of past and future events. Psychological Science, 19(8), 796–801.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Clore, G. L., & Huntsinger, J. R. (2007). How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 393–399.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences Lawrence Earlbaum Associates (pp. 20–26). NJ: Hillsdale.Google Scholar
  15. DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Chester, D. S., & Bushman, B. J. (2016). How often does currently felt emotion predict social behavior and judgment? A meta-analytic test of two theories. Emotion Review, 8(2), 136–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Five-year olds, but not chimpanzees, attempt to manage their reputations. PLoS ONE, 7(10), e48433.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Ferrante, D., Girotto, V., Stragà, M., & Walsh, C. (2013). Improving the past and the future: A temporal asymmetry in hypothetical thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions: Studies in emotion and social (interaction ed.). de La: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Gächter, S., & Schulz, J. F. (2016). Intrinsic honesty and the prevalence of rule violations across societies. Nature, 531(7595), 496–499.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35(7), 603–618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316(5827), 998–1002.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  25. Helzer, E. G., & Gilovich, T. (2012). Whatever is willed will be: A temporal asymmetry in attributions to will. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(10), 1235–1246.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. New York: General Learning Press.Google Scholar
  27. Jones, Edward E., Rock, L., Shaver, K. G., Goethals, G. R., & Ward, L. M. (1968). Pattern of performance and ability attribution: An unexpected primacy effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10(4), 317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jordan, J. J., Hoffman, M., Bloom, P., & Rand, D. G. (2016). Third-party punishment as a costly signal of trustworthiness. Nature, 530(7591), 473–476.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1121–1134.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Leach, C. W., & Cidam, A. (2015). When is shame linked to constructive approach orientation? A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(6), 983.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Mazar, N., Amir, O., & Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. Journal of Marketing Research, 45(6), 633–644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Morewedge, C. K., & Buechel, E. C. (2013). Motivated underpinnings of the impact bias in affective forecasts. Emotion, 13(6), 1023–1029.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Norem, J. K., & Cantor, N. (1986). Defensive pessimism: Harnessing anxiety as motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1208.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Pagliaro, S., Ellemers, N., Barreto, M., & Di Cesare, C. (2016). Once dishonest, always dishonest? The impact of perceived pervasiveness of moral evaluations of the self on motivation to restore a moral reputation. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 586.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pinker, S. (2011). The better angels of our nature: Why violence has declined. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
  36. Rand, D. G., & Nowak, M. A. (2013). Human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 413–425.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Rothbart, M., & Snyder, M. (1970). Confidence in the prediction and postdiction of an uncertain outcome. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 2(1), 38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(4), 296–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2003). Mood as information: 20 years later. Psychological Inquiry, 14(3–4), 296–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Seligman, M. E. P., Railton, P., Baumeister, R. F., & Sripada, C. (2013). Navigating into the future or driven by the past. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(2), 119–141.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Shepperd, J. A., Klein, W. M., Waters, E. A., & Weinstein, N. D. (2013). Taking stock of unrealistic optimism. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(4), 395–411.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sjåstad, H. (2019). Short-sighted greed? Focusing on the future promotes reputation-based generosity. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(2), 199–213.Google Scholar
  43. Suddendorf, T. (2013). The gap: The science of what separates us from other animals. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  44. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193–210.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Uhlmann, E. L., Pizarro, D. A., & Diermeier, D. (2015). A person-centered approach to moral judgment. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(1), 72–81.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1985). A theory of action identification. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  48. Vallacher, Robin R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they’re doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94(1), 3–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Van Bavel, J., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Brady, W. J., & Reinero, D. A. (2016). Contextual sensitivity in scientific reproducibility. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 6454–6459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Van Boven, L., & Ashworth, L. (2007). Looking forward, looking back: Anticipation is more evocative than retrospection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(2), 289–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Vonasch, A. J., Reynolds, T., Winegard, B. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2018). Death before dishonor: Incurring costs to protect moral reputation. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(5), 604–613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(5), 806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting knowing what to want. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 131–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Ybarra, O., Park, H., Stanik, C., & Lee, D. S. (2012). Self-judgment and reputation monitoring as a function of the fundamental dimensions, temporal perspective, and culture. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42(2), 200–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Zuckerman, M. (1979). Attribution of success and failure revisited, or: The motivational bias is alive and well in attribution theory. Journal of Personality, 47(2), 245–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.FAIR Insight TeamNorwegian School of Economics and SNF - Centre for Applied Research at NHHBergenNorway
  2. 2.University of Queensland and Florida State UniversityBrisbaneAustralia

Personalised recommendations