Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 43, Issue 1, pp 52–62 | Cite as

The attenuating effect of mortality salience on dishonest behavior

  • Simon SchindlerEmail author
  • Marc-André Reinhard
  • Sandra Dobiosch
  • Ina Steffan-Fauseweh
  • Galip Özdemir
  • Jeff Greenberg
Original Paper

Abstract

People are easily tempted to engage in dishonest behavior when an incentive is given and when full anonymity is provided. In the present work, we investigated existential threat as a motivational factor that might reduce dishonest behavior. Research based on terror management theory has found that mortality salience increases the motivation to fulfill salient values of one’s cultural worldview. Assuming the concept of honesty is important to human societies, we hypothesized that mortality salience will reduce dishonest behavior when the concept of honesty is salient. In two experiments, we assessed dishonesty under full anonymity by applying a die-under-the-cup paradigm with the expected value serving as a stochastic baseline for honest behavior. Both experiments provided support for our hypothesis. Given an incentive to cheat, when the concept of honesty was cognitively activated by a word-search puzzle (Study 1) or by the name of the dice game (i.e., “honest game”; Study 2), mortality salient participants showed not only less dishonest behavior but actually honest behavior.

Keywords

Dishonest behavior Cheating Mortality salience Terror management theory Honesty 

References

  1. Abeler, J., Becker, A., & Falk, A. (2014). Representative evidence on lying costs. Journal of Public Economics, 113, 96–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Abeler, J., Nosenzo, D., & Raymond, D. (2016). Preferences for truth-telling. CeDEx working paper no. 2016-13. Munich: Center for Economic Studies & Ifo Institute.Google Scholar
  3. Ariely, D. (2012). The honest truth about dishonesty. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
  4. Arndt, J., Cox, C. R., Goldenberg, J. L., Vess, M., Routledge, C., Cooper, D. P., & Cohen, F. (2009). Blowing in the (social) wind: Implications of extrinsic esteem contingencies for terror management and health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1191–1205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., & Cook, A. (2002). Mortality salience and the spreading activation of worldview-relevant constructs: Exploring the cognitive architecture of terror management. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 307–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Arndt, J., & Solomon, S. (2003). The control of death and the death of control: The effects of mortality salience, neuroticism, and worldview threat on the desire for control. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Batigalli, P., Dufwenberg, M., & Charness, G. (2013). Deception: The role of guilt. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 93, 227–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Becker, E. (1973). The denial of death. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  9. Becker, G. S. (1968). Crime and punishment: An economic approach. Journal of Political Economy, 76(2), 169–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bishin, B. G., Stevens, D., & Wilson, C. (2006). Character counts? Honesty and fairness in election 2000. Public Opinion Quarterly, 70, 235–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brandt, M. J., Ijzerman, H., Dijksterhuis, A., Farach, F. J., Geller, J., Giner-Sorolla, R., … Van’t Veer, A. (2014). The replication recipe: What makes for a convincing replication? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 217–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Burke, B. L., Martens, A., & Faucher, E. H. (2010). Two decades of terror management theory: A meta-analysis of mortality salience research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 155–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Castano, E., Yzerbyt, V., Paladino, M. P., & Sacchi, S. (2002). I belong, therefore, I exist: Ingroup identification, ingroup entitativity, and ingroup bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 135–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cialdini, R. B., Kallgren, C. A., & Reno, R. R. (1991). A focus theory of normative conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 201–234.Google Scholar
  15. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  16. Crandall, C. S., & Sherman, J. W. (2016). On the scientific superiority of conceptual replications for scientific progress. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 66, 93–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. DePaulo, B. M., Kashy, D. A., Kirkendol, S. E., Wyer, M. M., & Epstein, J. A. (1996). Lying in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  19. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149–1160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fischbacher, U., & Föllmi-Heusi, F. (2013). Lies in disguise—An experimental study on cheating. Journal of the European Economic Association, 11, 525–547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fritsche, I., Jonas, E., Kayser, D. N., & Koranyi, N. (2010). Existential threat and compliance with pro-environmental norms. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 30(1), 67–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gailliot, M. T., Stillman, T. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Maner, J. K., & Plant, E. A. (2008). Mortality salience increases adherence to salient norms and values. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 993–1003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Geißler, H., Schöpe, S., Klewes, J., Rauh, C., & von Alemann, U. (2013). Wertestudie 2013: Wie groß ist die Kluft zwischen dem Volk und seinen Vertretern? Köln: YouGov.Google Scholar
  24. Giannakakis, A. E., & Fritsche, I. (2011). Social identities, group norms, and threat: On the malleability of ingroup bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 82–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gneezy, U. (2005). Deception: the role of consequences. American Economic Review, 9, 384–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1986). The cause and consequences of a need for self-esteem: A terror management theory. In R. R. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private self (pp. 189–212). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Greenberg, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Chatel, D. (1992). Terror management and tolerance: Does mortality salience always intensify negative reactions to others who threaten one‘s worldview? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 212–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Harmon-Jones, E., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Simon, L. (1996). The effects of mortality salience on intergroup bias between minimal groups. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 677–681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. E. (1987). Social cognition and social perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 369–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hilbig, B. E., & Zettler, I. (2015). When the cat’s away, some mice will play: A basic trait account of dishonest behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 57, 72–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hirschberger, G., Florian, V., Mikulincer, M., Goldenberg, J. L., & Pyszczynski, T. (2002). Gender differences in the willingness to engage in risky behavior: A terror management perspective. Death Studies, 26, 117–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Horne, J. A., & Ostberg, O. (1975). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology, 4, 97–110.Google Scholar
  33. Houser, D., Vetter, S., & Winter, J. (2012). Fairness and cheating. European Economic Review, 56, 1645–1655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Jonas, E., Martens, A., Niesta, D., Fritsche, I., Sullivan, D., & Greenberg, J. (2008). Focus theory of normative conduct and terror management theory: The interactive impact of mortality salience and norm salience on social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1239–1251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Jonas, E., Schimel, J., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (2002). The Scrooge effect: Evidence that mortality salience increases prosocial attitudes and behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(10), 1342–1353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Jonas, E., Sullivan, D., & Greenberg, J. (2013). Generosity, greed, norms, and death—Differential effects of mortality salience on charitable behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 35, 47–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kelley, N. J., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2015). Mortality salience increases personal optimism among individuals higher in trait self-control. Motivation and Emotion, 39, 926–931.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kirchler, E., Hoelzl, E., & Wahl, I. (2008). Enforced versus voluntary tax compliance: The “slippery slope” framework. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29, 210–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Lambert, A. J., Eadeh, F. R., Peak, S. A., Scherer, L. D., Schott, J. P., & Slochower, J. M. (2014). Toward a greater understanding of the emotional dynamics of the mortality salience manipulation: Revisiting the “affect-free” claim of terror management research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 655–678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Liberman, V., Samuels, S. M., & Ross, L. (2004). The name of the game: Predictive power of reputations versus situational labels in determining prisoner’s dilemma game moves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1175–1185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Mazar, N., Amir, O., & Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. Journal of Marketing Research, 45, 633–644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Mazar, N., & Ariely, D. (2006). Dishonesty in everyday life and its policy implications. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25, 117–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. myMarktforschung.de (2016). Ehrlichkeit: Wie viel Pinocchio steckt in den Deutschen? Retrieved from https://www.mymarktforschung.de/studien/Studie-Ehrlichkeit-2016.pdf.
  44. Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Arndt, J., & Schimel, J. (2004). Why do people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 435–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rosenbaum, S. M., Billinger, S., & Stieglitz, N. (2014). Let’s be honest: A review of experimental evidence of honesty and truth-telling. Journal of Economic Psychology, 45, 181–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Lyon, D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory I: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 681–690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Schindler, S., & Pfattheicher, S. (2017). The frame of the game: Loss-framing increases dishonest behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 172–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Schindler, S., & Reinhard, M. A. (2015b). Catching the liar as a matter of justice: Effects of belief in a just world on deception detection accuracy and the moderating role of mortality salience. Personality and Individual Differences, 73, 105–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Schindler, S., & Reinhard, M. A. (2015c). Increasing skepticism toward potential liars: Effects of existential threat on veracity judgments and the moderating role of honesty norm activation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1312.Google Scholar
  50. Schindler, S., Reinhard, M. A., & Stahlberg, D. (2013). Tit for tat in the face of death: The effect of mortality salience on reciprocal behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 87–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Schindler, S., & Reinhard, M.-A. (2015a). A hero in the name of truth: Mortality salience increases heroic perceptions of Edward Snowden. International Journal of Psychological Studies, 7, 43–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Schindler, S., & Reinhard, M.-A. (2015d). When death is compelling: Door-in-the-face compliance under mortality salience. Social Psychology, 46, 352–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Schindler, S., Reinhard, M.-A., & Stahlberg, D. (2012). Mortality salience increases personal relevance of the norm of reciprocity. Psychological Reports, 111, 565–574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human values? Journal of Social Issues, 50, 19–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Serota, K. B., Levine, T. R., & Boster, F. J. (2010). The prevalence of lying in America: Three studies of self-reported lies. Human Communication Research, 36, 2–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Shalvi, S., Handgraaf, M. J., & De Dreu, C. K. (2011). Ethical manoeuvring: Why people avoid both major and minor lies. British Journal of Management, 22, 16–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Small, M. V., & Dickie, L. (1999). A cinematograph of moral principles: Critical values for contemporary business and society. Journal of Management Development, 18, 628–638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (2004). The cultural animal: Twenty years of terror management theory and research. In J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of experimental existential psychology (pp. 13–34). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  59. Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (2015) The worm at the core: On the role of death in life. London: Random House.Google Scholar
  60. Taubman Ben-Ari, O., & Findler, L. (2003). Reckless driving and gender: An examination of a terror management theory explanation. Death Studies, 27, 603–618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Taubman Ben-Ari, O., Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1999). The impact of mortality salience on reckless driving: A test of terror management mechanisms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 35–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Vess, M., Arndt, J., Cox, C. R., Routledge, C., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2009). Exploring the existential function of religion: The effect of religious fundamentalism and mortality salience on faith-based medical refusals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 334–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). Affects separable and inseparable: On the hierarchical arrangement of the negative affects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 489–505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1063–1070.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Weber, M., & Ruch, W. (2012). The role of character strengths in adolescent romantic relationships: An initial study on partner selection and mates’ life satisfaction. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 1537–1546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Welsh, D. T., Ordóñez, L. D., Snyder, D. G., & Christian, M. S. (2015). The slippery slope: How small ethical transgressions pave the way for larger future transgressions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 114–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Yen, C. L., & Cheng, C. P. (2013). Researcher effects on mortality salience research: A meta-analytic moderator analysis. Death Studies, 37, 636–652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Zaleskiewicz, T., Gasiorowska, A., Kesebir, P., Luszczynska, A., & Pyszczynski, T. (2013). Money and the fear of death: The symbolic power of money as an existential anxiety buffer. Journal of Economic Psychology, 36, 55–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of KasselKasselGermany
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of ArizonaTucsonUSA

Personalised recommendations