Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 43, Issue 2, pp 313–324 | Cite as

Goals, emotions, and the effort to be responsive during couple interactions

  • Wei-Fang Lin
  • Courtney L. Gosnell
  • Shelly L. GableEmail author
Original Paper
  • 185 Downloads

Abstract

Previous research has shown that perceived responsiveness benefits psychological and relationship well-being. Perceived responsiveness is, at least partly, a function of how responsiveness the partner intended to be during the interaction. Relatively little is known about what factors make people intend to be more or less responsive to their partners. In two studies, we examined whether individuals’ experienced emotion and underlying relationship goals were linked to their intended responsiveness. Across two studies, both experienced positive emotion and approach goals predicted higher intended responsiveness, whereas experienced negative emotions predicted lower intended responsiveness. In addition, Study 2 also showed that people with strong approach goals intended to provide more responsiveness when they experienced more positive emotions. In contrast, the negative association between experienced negative emotions and intended responsiveness was stronger for low avoidance individuals than for high avoidance individuals. Our findings highlight that experienced positive and negative emotions may provide different information relavant to an individuals’ intended responsiveness depending on their relationship goals.

Keywords

Approach and avoidance motivation Close relationship Emotions Responsiveness 

Notes

Funding

Funding was provided by Directorate for Social, Behavioral and Economic Sciences (Grant No. 0444129).

References

  1. Burgoon, J. K., Dillman, L., & Stern, L. A. (1995). Interpersonal adaptation: Dyadic interaction patterns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Canevello, A., & Crocker, J. (2010). Creating good relationships: Responsiveness, relationship quality, and interpersonal goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 78–106.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018186.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Caprariello, P. A., & Reis, H. T. (2010). Perceived partner responsiveness minimizes defensive reactions to failure. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(4), 365–372.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550610391914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Carlson, M., Charlin, V., & Miller, N. (1988). Positive mood and helping behavior: A test of six hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(2), 211–229.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.55.2.211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Caughlin, J. P. (2010). A multiple goals theory of personal relationships: Conceptual integration and program overview. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 27(6), 824–848.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407510373262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Courtright, J. (2013). Observing and analyzing communication behavior. New York: Peter Lang Publishing Inc.Google Scholar
  7. Elliot, A. J., Gable, S. L., & Mapes, R. R. (2006). Approach and avoidance motivation in the social domain. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 378–391.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205282153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 300–319.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19(3), 313–332.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930441000238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gable, P. A., Poole, B. D., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2015). Anger perceptually and conceptually narrows cognitive scope. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(1), 163–174.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gable, S. L. (2006). Approach and avoidance social motives and goals. Journal of Personality, 74, 175–222.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00373.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gable, S. L., Gonzaga, G. C., & Strachman, A. (2006). Will you be there for me when things go right? Supportive responses to positive event disclosures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 904–917.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gable, S. L., Gosnell, C. L., Maisel, N. C., & Strachman, A. (2012). Safely testing the alarm: Close others’ responses to personal positive events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 963–981.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gable, S. L., & Poore, J. (2008). Which thoughts count? Algorithms for evaluating satisfaction in relationships. Psychological Science, 19, 1030–1036.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02195.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Behavioral activation and inhibition in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(6), 1135–1149.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.6.1135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., Impett, E. A., & Asher, E. R. (2004). What do you do when things go right? The intrapersonal and interpersonal benefits of sharing positive events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 228–245.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Impett, E. A., Gordon, A. M., Kogan, A., Oveis, C., Gable, S. L., & Keltner, D. (2010). Moving toward more perfect unions: Daily and long-term consequences of approach and avoidance goals in romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 948–963.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Impett, E. A., Strachman, A., Finkel, E. J., & Gable, S. L. (2008). Maintaining sexual desire in intimate relationships: The importance of approach goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 808–823.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jones, S. M., & Wirtz, J. G. (2006). How does the comforting process work? An empirical test of an appraisal-based model of comforting. Human Communication Research, 32(3), 217–243.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2006.00274.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Keck, K. L., & Samp, J. A. (2007). The dynamic nature of goals and message production as revealed in a sequential analysis of conflict interactions. Human Communication Research, 33(1), 27–47.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2007.00287.x.Google Scholar
  22. Keltner, D., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 13(5), 467–480.  https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (1998). Emotion, social function, and psychopathology. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 320–342.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Laurenceau, J.-P., Barrett, L. F., & Pietromonaco, P. R. (1998). Intimacy as an interpersonal process: The importance of self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and perceived partner responsiveness in interpersonal exchanges. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1238–1251.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.74.5.1238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Maisel, N. C., & Gable, S. L. (2009). The paradox of received social support: The importance of responsiveness. Psychological Science, 20, 928–932.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02388.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Maisel, N. C., Gable, S. L., & Strachman, A. (2008). Responsive behaviors in good times and in bad. Personal Relationships, 15, 317–338.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2008.00201.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437–448.  https://doi.org/10.3102/10769986031004437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Raudenbush, S. W., Byrk, A. S., Chenong, Y. F., & Congdon, R. T. (2004). HLM 6: Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling. Chicago: Scientific Software International.Google Scholar
  29. Reis, H. T., Clark, M. S., & Holmes, J. G. (2004). Perceived partner responsiveness as an organizing construct in the study of intimacy and closeness. In D. Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), The handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 201–225). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  30. Reis, H. T., & Gable, S. L. (2015). Responsiveness. Current Opinion in Psychology, 1, 67–71.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.01.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Reis, H. T., & Shaver, P. (1988). Intimacy as an interpersonal process. In S. Duck & D. F. Hay (Eds.), Handbook of personal relationships: Theory, research, and interventions (pp. 367–389). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  32. Updegraff, J. A., Gable, S. L., & Taylor, S. E. (2004). What makes experiences satisfying? The interaction of approach-avoidance motivations and emotions in well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(3), 496–504.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.3.496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyChung Yuan Christian UniversityTaoyuanTaiwan
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyPace UniversityNew YorkUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CaliforniaSanta BarbaraUSA

Personalised recommendations