Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 42, Issue 6, pp 885–895 | Cite as

Memory performance in older adults: Experimental evidence for the indirect effect of memory self-efficacy on processing efficiency through worry

  • Marine Beaudoin
Original Paper
  • 69 Downloads

Abstract

This research examined the mediating role of worry in the relationship between memory self-efficacy (MSE) and processing efficiency during episodic memory performance in older adults. We conducted an experimental study in which we manipulated older adults’ MSE using a false feedback paradigm. The memory task was adapted to provide an indicator of performance effectiveness (recall score) and an indicator of processing efficiency (ratio of recall score to number of encoding trials needed). We measured participants’ worry and emotionality before task completion. The causal impact of MSE condition (high vs. low MSE) on processing efficiency was significantly mediated through worry, with lower MSE leading to increased worry, which predicted impaired processing efficiency. This study provides the first experimental evidence for the mediating effect of worry on the causal impact of MSE on processing efficiency in older adults when they perform challenging memory tasks. We discuss implications for the clinical diagnosis of cognitive impairment and for age-related disengagement from everyday cognitive activities.

Keywords

Memory self-efficacy Performance effectiveness Processing efficiency Anxiety Worry Aging 

Notes

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Elodie Falque, Adeline Guegan, and Lucile Letscher for their assistance with participants recruitment and data collection.

Compliance with ethical standards

Ethical approval

All procedures involving human participants were carried out in accordance with the Institution’s and/or National Research Committee’s ethical standards, and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and subsequent amendments, or with comparable ethical standards.

References

  1. Adam, S., Van der Linden, M., Ivanoiu, A., Juillerat, A. C., Bechet, S., & Salmon, E. (2007). Optimization of encoding specificity for the diagnosis of early AD: The RI-48 task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 477–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andreoletti, C., Veratti, B. W., & Lachman, M. E. (2006). Age differences in the relationship between anxiety and recall. Aging and Mental Health, 10, 265–271.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13607860500409773.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bandura, A. (1989). Regulation of cognitive processes through perceived self-efficacy. Developmental Psychology, 25, 729–735.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.5.729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bandura, A. (1993). Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development and functioning. Educational Psychologist, 28, 117–148.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep2802_3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bandura, A. (2003). Auto-efficacité: Le sentiment d’efficacité personnelle [Self-efficacy: The sense of personal efficacy]. Bruxelles: De Boeck Diffusion.Google Scholar
  7. Bandura, A. (2012). On the functional properties of perceived self-efficacy revisited. Journal of Management, 38, 9–44.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206311410606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Beaudoin, M., & Desrichard, O. (2009). Validation of a short french state test worry and emotionality scale. International Review of Social Psychology, 22, 79–104.Google Scholar
  9. Beaudoin, M., & Desrichard, O. (2011). Are memory self-efficacy and memory performance related? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 211–241.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022106.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Beaudoin, M., & Desrichard, O. (2017). Memory self-efficacy and memory performance in older adults: The mediating role of task persistence. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 76, 23–33.  https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Beaudreau, S. A., & O’Hara, R. (2009). The association of anxiety and depressive symptoms with cognitive performance in community-dwelling older adults. Psychology and Aging, 24, 507–512.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016035.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Berggren, N., & Derakshan, N. (2013). Attentional control deficits in trait anxiety: Why you see them and why you don’t. Biological Psychology, 92, 440–446.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Berry, J. M. (1999). Memory self-efficacy in its social cognitive context. In T. M. Hess & F. Blanchard-Fields (Eds.), Social cognition and aging (pp. 69–96). San Diego, CA: Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Berry, J. M., Hastings, E. C., West, R. L., Courtney, L., & Cavanaugh, J. C. (2010). Memory aging: Deficits, beliefs, and interventions. In J. C. Cavanaugh, C. K. Cavanaugh, J. M. Berry & R. L. West (Eds.), Aging in America. Psychological aspects (Vol. 1, pp. 255–299). Barbara, CA: Praeger/ABC-CLIO.Google Scholar
  15. Berry, J. M., West, R. L., & Dennehey, D. M. (1989). Reliability and validity of the Memory Self-Efficacy Questionnaire. Developmental Psychology, 25, 701–713.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.5.701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Bouazzaoui, B., Isingrini, M., Fay, S., Angel, L., Vanneste, S., Clarys, D., & Taconnat, L. (2010). Aging and self-reported internal and external memory strategy uses: The role of executive functioning. Acta Psychologica, 135, 59–66.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.05.007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Calvo, M. G., Ramos, P. M., & Estevez, A. (1992). Test anxiety and comprehension efficiency: The role of prior knowledge and working memory deficits. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping: An International Journal, 5, 125–138.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10615809208250492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cassady, J. C., & Johnson, R. E. (2002). Cognitive test anxiety and academic performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 270–295.  https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.2001.1094.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (2009). Anxiety, processing efficiency, and cognitive performance: New developments from attentional control theory. European Psychologist, 14, 168–176.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040.14.2.168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dixon, R. A., & Hultsch, D. F. (1983). Metamemory and memory for text relationships in adulthood: A cross-validation study. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 689–694.  https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/38.6.689.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Dixon, R. A., Hultsch, D. F., & Hertzog, C. (1988). The Metamemory In Adulthood (MIA) questionnaire. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24, 671–688.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Elfgren, C., Gustafson, L., Vestberg, S., & Passant, U. (2010). Subjective memory complaints, neuropsychological performance and psychiatric variables in memory clinic attendees: A 3-year follow-up study. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 51, 110–114.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2010.02.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Endler, N. S., Edwards, J. M., & Vitelli, R. (1991). Endler multidimensional anxiety scales (EMAS): Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  24. Eysenck, M. W. (1976). Arousal, learning, and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 389–404.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.83.3.389.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409–434.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939208409696.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Eysenck, M. W., & Derakshan, N. (2011). New perspectives in attentional control theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 955–960.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Gardiner, M., Luszcz, M. A., & Bryan, J. (1997). The manipulation and measurement of task-specific memory self-efficacy in younger and older adults. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 209–227.  https://doi.org/10.1080/016502597384839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Goodwin, L. D., & Leech, N. L. (2006). Understanding correlation: Factors that affect the size of r. The Journal of Experimental Education, 74, 249–266.  https://doi.org/10.3200/JEXE.74.3.249-266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Grober, E., & Buschke, H. (1987). Genuine memory deficits in dementia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 3, 13–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408–420.  https://doi.org/10.1080/03637750903310360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  33. Hess, T. M. (2005). Memory and aging in context. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 383–406.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.3.383.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Hoffman, B., & Schraw, G. (2009). The influence of self-efficacy and working memory capacity on problem-solving efficiency. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 91–100.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2008.08.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hong, E. (1998). Differential stability of individual differences in state and trait test anxiety. Learning and Individual Differences, 10, 51.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S1041-6080(99)80142-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hong, E. (1999). Test anxiety, perceived test difficulty, and test performance: Temporal patterns of their effects. Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 431.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S1041-6080(99)80012-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Howell, D. C. (1998). Méthodes statistiques en sciences humaines [Statistical methods in the social sciences]. Paris: De Boeck Université.Google Scholar
  38. Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J., & Dixon, R. A. (1999). Use it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychology and Aging, 14, 245–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Hummert, M. L. (2011). Age stereotype and aging. In K. W. Schaie & S. L. Willis (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (7th ed., pp. 249–262). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Jacobs, B., Prentice-Dunn, S., & Rogers, R. W. (1984). Understanding persistence: An interface of control theory and self-efficacy theory. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 5, 333–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Jonker, C., Smits, C. H. M., & Deeg, D. J. H. (1997). Affect-related metamemory and memory performance in a population-based sample of older adults. Educational Gerontology, 23, 115–128.  https://doi.org/10.1080/0360127970230203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kim, S. H., & Rocklin, T. (1994). The temporal patterns of worry and emotionality and their differential effects on test performance. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 7, 117–130.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10615809408249339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., & Fricker, S. S. (1994). The effects of self-beliefs on repeated efforts to remember. Social Cognition, 12, 249–261.  https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.1994.12.4.249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Lachman, M. E., Neupert, S. D., & Agrigoroaei, S. (2011). The relevance of control beliefs for health and aging. In K. W. Schaie & S. L. Willis (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (7th ed.). London: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  45. Li, J., Nilsson, L. G., & Wu, Z. (2004). Effects of age and anxiety on episodic memory: Selectivity and variability. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 45, 123–129.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2004.00387.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Marquez, D. X., Jerome, G. J., McAuley, E., Snook, E. M., & Canaklisova, S. (2002). Self-efficacy manipulation and state anxiety responses to exercise in low active women. Psychology and Health, 17, 783–791.  https://doi.org/10.1080/0887044021000054782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. McDougall, G. J. (1995). Memory self-efficacy and strategy use in successful elders. Educational Gerontology, 21, 357–373.  https://doi.org/10.1080/0360127950210406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Minor, S. W., & Gold, S. R. (1985). Worry and emotionality components of test anxiety. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 82–85.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_15.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Molden, J., & Maxfield, M. (2016). The impact of aging stereotypes on dementia worry. European Journal of Ageing, 14, 29–37.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10433-016-0378-z.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  50. Morris, L. W., Davis, M. A., & Hutchings, C. H. (1981). Cognitive and emotional components of anxiety: Literature review and a revised worry-emotionality scale. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 541–555.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.73.4.541.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Onwuegbuzie, A., & Seaman, M. A. (1995). The effect of time constraints and statistics test anxiety on test performance in a statistics course. The Journal of Experimental Education, 63, 115–124.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.1995.9943816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Pálsson, S., Johansson, B., Berg, S., & Skoog, I. (2000). A population study on the influence of depression on neuropsychological functioning in 85-year-olds. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 101, 185–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Potter, G. G., & Hartman, M. (2006). Response inhibition and everyday memory complaints in older adult women. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21, 1115–1120.  https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1615.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Potvin, O., Bergua, V., Meillon, C., Le Goff, M., Bouisson, J., Dartigues, J.-F., & Amieva, H. (2013). State anxiety and cognitive functioning in older adults. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21, 915–924.  https://doi.org/10.1097/JGP.0b013e31826576b4.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 717–731.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206553.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Sindi, S., Fiocco, A. J., Juster, R.-P., Pruessner, J., & Lupien, S. J. (2013). When we test, do we stress? Impact of the testing environment on cortisol secretion and memory performance in older adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 1388–1396.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.004.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  59. Valentijn, S. A. M., Hill, R. D., Van Hooren, S. A. H., Bosma, H., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Jolles, J., & Ponds, R. (2006). Memory self-efficacy predicts memory performance: Results from a 6-year follow-up study. Psychology and Aging, 21, 165–172.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.165.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Vanier, M., & Lemyze, C. (1994). Rivermead behavioral memory test (2nd ed.). [Measurement instrument]. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.Google Scholar
  61. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-X: Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule—Expanded form. University of Iowa, Department of Psychology Publication.Google Scholar
  62. Watters, P. A., Martin, F., & Schreter, Z. (1997). Caffeine and cognitive performance: The nonlinear Yerkes–Dodson Law. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 12, 249–257. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1077(199705/06)12:3<249::AID-HUP865>3.0.CO;2-J.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Wechsler, D. (2001). MEM-III: Manuel de l’Echelle Clinique de Mémoire (3 ed.) [WMS-III: Wechsler Memory Scale (3rd ed.)]. Paris: Les Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.Google Scholar
  64. West, R. L., Bagwell, D. K., & King, A. (2006). The impact of group size on memory and memory beliefs: A methodological report. Experimental Aging Research, 32, 297–316.  https://doi.org/10.1080/03610730600699092.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. West, R. L., Boatwright, L. K., & Schleser, R. (1984). The link between memory performance, self-assessment, and affective status. Experimental Aging Research, 10, 197–200.  https://doi.org/10.1080/03610738408258464.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Wetherell, J. L., Reynolds, C. A., Gatz, M., & Pedersen, N. L. (2002). Anxiety, cognitive performance, and cognitive decline in normal aging. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57B, 246–255.  https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/57.3.P246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Wilson, R. S., Scherr, P. A., Schneider, J. A., Tang, Y., & Bennett, D. A. (2007). The relation of cognitive activity to risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 69, 1911–1920.  https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000271087.67782.c.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Zeidner, M. (1998). Test anxiety: The state of the art. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Univ. Grenoble Alpes, Univ. Savoie Mont Blanc, LIP/PC2SGrenobleFrance

Personalised recommendations