Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 40, Issue 6, pp 862–877 | Cite as

The effects of implicit and explicit affiliation motives on vagal activity in motive-relevant situations

  • Elisabeth PresteleEmail author
  • Friederike X. R. Gerstenberg
  • Birk Hagemeyer
  • Fay C. M. Geisler
Original Paper

Abstract

We investigated the independent and interactive effects of the implicit need for affiliation (nAFF) and the explicit self-attributed need for affiliation (sanAFF) on parasympathetic vagal activity (indexed via heart rate variability) in three motive-relevant situations: in a socioevaluative stress situation (N = 49), in a socially ambiguous situation (N = 50), and during socially supported recovery from stress (both subsamples). Vagal activity has been linked with self-regulation and social engagement. Vagal withdrawal has been found to accompany stress responses, whereas vagal advance has been found to accompany attenuated stress and affiliative behavior. Response surface analyses in the current study revealed additive but opposite effects on vagal activity for nAFF (vagal advance) and sanAFF (vagal withdrawal) during the socioevaluative stress situation, high nAFF and low sanAFF incongruence predicted vagal withdrawal in the socially ambiguous situation, and sanAFF predicted vagal advance during socially supported recovery from stress. We suggest that assessing reactions to motive-relevant stress situations represents a profitable approach for investigating the differential effects of implicit and explicit motives.

Keywords

Implicit need for affiliation Explicit need for affiliation Heart rate variability Response Surface analysis Polyvagal theory 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Christine Altstötter-Gleich and Anette Hiemisch for valuable comments on an earlier version of this article.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in the current study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

References

  1. Baumann, N., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Striving for unwanted goals: stress-dependent discrepancies between explicit and implicit achievement motives reduce subjective well-being and increase psychosomatic symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(5), 781–799. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.5.781.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497–529.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Brewer, S., & Klein, J. D. (2006). Type of positive interdependence and affiliation motive in an asynchronous, collaborative learning environment. Educational Technology Research and Development, 54(4), 331–354. doi: 10.1007/s11423-006-9603-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brunstein, J. C., & Hoyer, S. (2002). Implizites versus explizites Leistungsstreben: Befunde zur unabhängigkeit zweier motivationssysteme [Implicit versus explicit achievement strivings: Empirical evidence of the independence of two motivational systems]. Zeitschrift Für Pädagogische Psychologie, 16(1), 51–62. doi: 10.1024//1010-0652.16.1.51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässman, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(2), 494–508.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Burnham, K. P., & Anderson, D. R. (2002). Model selection and multimodel inference: A practical information-theoretic approach. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media.Google Scholar
  7. Crowley, O. V., McKinley, P. S., Burg, M. M., Schwartz, J. E., Ryff, C. D., Weinstein, M., et al. (2011). The interactive effect of change in perceived stress and trait anxiety on vagal recovery from cognitive challenge. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 82(3), 225–232. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.09.002.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Denson, T. F., Grisham, J. R., & Moulds, M. L. (2011). Cognitive reappraisal increases heart rate variability in response to an anger provocation. Motivation and Emotion, 35(1), 14–22. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9201-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin, 130(3), 355–391. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Dufner, M., Arslan, R. C., Hagemeyer, B., Schönbrodt, F. D., & Denissen, J. J. A. (2015). Affective contingencies in the affiliative domain: Physiological assessment, associations with the affiliation motive, and prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(4), 662–676. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000025.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Edwards, J. R. (2002). Alternatives to difference scores: Polynomial regression analysis and response surface methodology. In F. Drasgow & N. Schmitt (Eds.), Measuring and analyzing behavior in organizations: Advances in measurement and data analysis (pp. 350–400). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  13. Egizio, V. B., Jennings, J. R., Christie, I. C., Sheu, L. K., Matthews, K. A., & Gianaros, P. J. (2008). Cardiac vagal activity during psychological stress varies with social functioning in older women. Psychophysiology, 45(6), 1046–1054. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00698.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Elliot, A. J., Gable, S. L., & Mapes, R. R. (2006). Approach and avoidance motivation in the social domain. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(3), 378–391. doi: 10.1177/0146167205282153.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Ellis, B., Jackson, J., & Boyce, W. (2006). The stress response systems: Universality and adaptive individual differences. Developmental Review, 26(2), 175–212. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2006.02.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Exline, R. V. (1963). Explorations in the process of person perception: Visual interaction in relation to competition, sex, and need for affiliation. Journal of Personality, 31(1), 1–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1963.tb01836.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. French, E. G., & Chadwick, I. (1956). Some characteristics in affiliation motivation. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53(3), 296–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Grenier, S., Barrette, A. M., & Ladouceur, R. (2005). Intolerance of uncertainty and intolerance of ambiguity: Similarities and differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(3), 593–600. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hagemeyer, B., Neberich, W., Asendorpf, J. B., & Neyer, F. J. (2013). Congruence of implicit and explicit communal motives predicts the quality and stability of couple relationships. Journal of Personality, 81(4), 390–402. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12016.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Het, S., Rohleder, N., Schoofs, D., Kirschbaum, C., & Wolf, O. T. (2009). Neuroendocrine and psychometric evaluation of a placebo version of the “Trier social stress test”. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(7), 1075–1086. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.008.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Hofer, J., & Busch, H. (2011). Satisfying one’s needs for competence and relatedness: consequent domain-specific well-being depends on strength of implicit motives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(9), 1147–1158. doi: 10.1177/0146167211408329.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Hofer, J., Chasiotis, A., & Campos, D. (2006). Congruence between social values and implicit motives: effects on life satisfaction across three cultures. European Journal of Personality, 20(4), 305–324. doi: 10.1002/per.590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Inzlicht, M., Bartholow, B. D., & Hirsh, J. B. (2015). Emotional foundations of cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(3), 126–132. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.01.004.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Inzlicht, M., & Legault, L. (2014). How distress underlies effective self-control (and unites diverse social psychological phenomena). In J. P. Forgas & E. Harmon-Jones (Eds.), Motivation and its regulation: The control within (pp. 115–132). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  25. Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K.-M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The “Trier social stress test” - a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28(1–2), 76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Köllner, M. G., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2014). Meta-analytic evidence of low convergence between implicit and explicit measures of the needs for achievement, affiliation, and power. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 826. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00826.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Kordik, A., Eska, K., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2012). Implicit need for affiliation is associated with increased corrugator activity in a non-positive, but not in a positive social interaction. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(5), 604–608. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.05.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lawrence, M. A. (2013). ez: Easy analysis and visualization of factorial experiments (R package version 4.2-2). Retrieved from http://cran.r-project.org/package=ez.
  29. Lazarus, R. S. (2006). Stress and emotion: A new synthesis. New York: Springer Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  30. Lundqvist, C., & Raglin, J. S. (2014). The relationship of basic need satisfaction, motivational climate and personality to well-being and stress patterns among elite athletes: An explorative study. Motivation and Emotion, 39(2), 237–246. doi: 10.1007/s11031-014-9444-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Maslow, A. H. (1958). A dynamic theory of human motivation. In M. Stacey & C. L. DeMartino (Eds.), Understanding human motivation (pp. 26–47). Cleveland, OH: H. Allen Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. McAdams, D. P., & Constantian, C. A. (1983). Intimacy and affiliation motives in daily living: An experience sampling analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(4), 851–861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. McAdams, D. P., Jackson, R. J., & Kirshnit, C. (1984). Looking, laughing, and smiling in dyads as a function of intimacy motivation and reciprocity. Journal of Personality, 52(3), 261–273. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1984.tb00881.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. McAdams, D. P., & Powers, J. (1981). Themes of intimacy in behavior and thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(3), 573–587. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.40.3.573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human motivation. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  36. McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96(4), 690–702. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. McClelland, D. C., Patel, V., Stier, D., & Brown, D. (1987). The relationship of affiliative arousal to dopamine release. Motivation and Emotion, 11(1), 51–66. doi: 10.1007/BF00992213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. McLain, D. L. (1993). The MSTAT-I: A new measure of an individual’s tolerance for ambiguity. Educational and psychological measurement, 53, 183–189. doi: 10.1177/0013164493053001020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Mehrabian, A., & Ksionzky, S. (1974). A theory of affiliation. Lexington, Mass.: Heath.Google Scholar
  40. Meuwly, N., Bodenmann, G., Germann, J., Bradbury, T. N., Ditzen, B., & Heinrichs, M. (2012). Dyadic coping, insecure attachment, and cortisol stress recovery following experimentally induced stress. Journal of Family Psychology : JFP, 26(6), 937–947. doi: 10.1037/a0030356.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Meyer, R. D., & Dalal, R. S. (2009). Situational strength as a means of conceptualizing context. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 2, 99–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Mezzacappa, E. S., Kelsey, R. M., Katkin, E. S., & Sloan, R. P. (2001). Vagal rebound and recovery from psychological stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63(4), 650–657.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Niskanen, J. P., Tarvainen, M. P., Ranta-Aho, P. O., & Karjalainen, P. A. (2004). Software for advanced HRV analysis. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, 76(1), 73–81. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2004.03.004.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Obradović, J., Bush, N. R., & Boyce, W. T. (2011). The interactive effect of marital conflict and stress reactivity on externalizing and internalizing symptoms: The role of laboratory stressors. Development and Psychopathology, 23(1), 101–114. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000672.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Penley, J., & Tomaka, J. (2002). Associations among the big five, emotional responses, and coping with acute stress. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(7), 1215–1228. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00087-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Porges, S. W. (2001). The polyvagal theory: Phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42(2), 123–146. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00162-3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 116–143. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Porges, S. W., & Furman, S. A. (2011). The early development of the autonomic nervous system provides a neural platform for social behaviour: A polyvagal perspective. Infant and Child Development, 20, 106–118. doi: 10.1002/icd.688.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  49. Quested, E., Bosch, J. A., Burns, V. E., Cumming, J., Ntoumanis, N., & Duda, J. L. (2011). Basic psychological need satisfaction, stress-related appraisals, and dancers’ cortisol and anxiety responses. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33(6), 828–846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Santiago-Rivera, A. L., & Bernstein, B. L. (1996). Affiliation, achievement and life events: Contributors to stress appraisals in college men and women. Personality and Individual Differences, 21(3), 411–419. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(96)00076-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Schönbrodt, F. (2007). Testing fit patterns with polynomial regression models. Germany: Munich.Google Scholar
  52. Schönbrodt, F. D. (2016). RSA: An R package for response surface analysis (version 0.9.10). Retrieved from http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/RSA/index.html.
  53. Schönbrodt, F. D., & Gerstenberg, F. X. R. (2012). An IRT analysis of motive questionnaires: The unified motive scales. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(6), 725–742. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.08.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Schüler, J., Job, V., Fröhlich, S. M., & Brandstätter, V. (2008). A high implicit affiliation motive does not always make you happy: A corresponding explicit motive and corresponding behavior are further needed. Motivation and Emotion, 32(3), 231–242. doi: 10.1007/s11031-008-9096-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Schüler, J., Job, V., Frölich, S. M., & Brandstätter, V. (2009). Dealing with a “hidden stressor”: Emotional disclosure as a coping strategy to overcome the negative effects of motive incongruence on health. Stress and Health, 25(3), 221–233. doi: 10.1002/smi.1241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Schultheiss, O. C. (2007). A biobehavioral model of implicit power motivation arousal, reward, and frustration. In E. Harmon-Jones & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Social neuroscience: Integrating biological and psychological explanations of social behavior (pp. 176–196). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  57. Schultheiss, O. C. (2008). Implicit motives. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 603–633). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  58. Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2002). Inhibited power motivation and persuasive communication: A lens model analysis. Journal of Personality, 70(4), 553–582. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.05014.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Schultheiss, O. C., & Pang, J. S. (2007). Measuring implicit motives. In R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley, & R. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 322–344). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  60. Schwerdtfeger, A. R., & Schlagert, H. (2011). The conjoined effect of naturalistic perceived available support and enacted support on cardiovascular reactivity during a laboratory stressor. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 42(1), 64–78. doi: 10.1007/s12160-011-9272-2.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Shewchuk, R. M., Elliott, T. R., MacNair-Semands, R. R., & Harkins, S. (1999). Trait influences on stress appraisal and coping: An evaluation of alternative frameworks. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29(4), 685–704. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1999.tb02019.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Sokolowski, K. (1986). Kognitionen und emotionen in anschlussthematischen situationen [Cognition and emotion in affiliative situations]. Wuppertal: Bergische Universität (Unpublished dissertation).Google Scholar
  63. Sokolowski, K. (1992). Entwicklung eines verfahrens zur messung des anschlussmotivs [The development of a measure for the motive to affiliate]. Diagnostica, 38, 1–17.Google Scholar
  64. Stewart, J. C., Janicki, D. L., & Kamarck, T. W. (2006). Cardiovascular reactivity to and recovery from psychological challenge as predictors of 3-year change in blood pressure. Health Psychology, 25(1), 111–118. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.1.111.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Task force of the european society of cardiology and the North American society of pacing and electrophysiology. (1996). Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiology interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation, 93, 1043–1065. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Tong, E. M. W. (2010). Personality influences in appraisal-emotion relationships: The role of neuroticism. Journal of Personality, 78(2), 393–417. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00620.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Wegner, M., Schüler, J., & Budde, H. (2014). The implicit affiliation motive moderates cortisol responses to acute psychosocial stress in high school students. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 48, 162–168. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.06.013.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Weinberger, J., Cotler, T., & Fishman, D. (2010). The duality of affiliative motivation. In O. Schultheiss & J. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 71–88). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Winter, D. G. (1994). Manual for scoring motive imagery in running text (4th ed.). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan.Google Scholar
  70. Winter, D. G. (1996). Personality: Analysis and interpretation of lives. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  71. Wirth, M. M., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2006). Effects of affiliation arousal (hope of closeness) and affiliation stress (fear of rejection) on progesterone and cortisol. Hormones and Behavior, 50(5), 786–795. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.08.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Yang, J., Yang, Y., Li, H., Hou, Y., Qi, M., Guan, L., et al. (2014). Correlation between self-esteem and stress response in Chinese college students: The mediating role of the need for social approval. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 212–217. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.049.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Elisabeth Prestele
    • 1
    Email author
  • Friederike X. R. Gerstenberg
    • 2
  • Birk Hagemeyer
    • 3
  • Fay C. M. Geisler
    • 4
  1. 1.University of Koblenz-LandauLandauGermany
  2. 2.Technische Universität MünchenMunichGermany
  3. 3.Friedrich-Schiller-Universität JenaJenaGermany
  4. 4.University of GreifswaldGreifswaldGermany

Personalised recommendations