Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 39, Issue 3, pp 384–391 | Cite as

What you want to avoid is what you see: Social avoidance motivation affects the interpretation of emotional faces

  • Jana NikitinEmail author
  • Alexandra M. Freund
Original Paper

Abstract

This study investigated the effects of habitual social approach and avoidance motivation on the classification of facial expressions of different visual clarity. Participants (N = 78) categorized partially masked emotional faces expressing either anger or happiness as positive or negative. Participants generally tended to interpret the facial expressions in a positive way. This positivity effect was reduced when persons were highly avoidance motivated. Social avoidance motivation predicted fewer positive and more negative interpretations in the least visible condition that provided extremely little information on the facial expression. Thus, people high in social avoidance motivation are likely to have anticipated angry faces as the facial stimuli offered only minimal information. The results for social approach motivation did not reach statistical significance. To conclude, it seems that persons who are most afraid of having negative social interactions (i.e., those high in social avoidance motivation), anticipate and interpret social information in the most negative way, which could lead to the reinforcement of the avoidance motivation.

Keywords

Social motivation Approach Avoidance Emotional faces Social-information processing 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Grant of the funding by Suzanne and Hans Biäsch Foundation for Applied Psychology, Switzerland (principle investigator: Jana Nikitin). We thank the Life-Management team for helpful discussions of the work reported in this paper.

References

  1. Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. (1974). Infant–mother attachment and social development: Socialisation as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In M. J. M. Richards (Ed.), The integration of a child into a social world (pp. 99–135). London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  2. Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.117.3.497.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Blanchard-Fields, F., Stein, R., & Watson, T. L. (2004). Age differences in emotion–regulation strategies in handling everyday problems. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59, 261–269. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.6.P261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1999). The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 839–855. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.76.5.839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cacioppo, J. T., & Hawkley, L. C. (2005). People thinking about people: The vicious cycle of being a social outcast in one’s own mind. In K. D. Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 91–108). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  6. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.76.6.893.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Downey, G., & Feldman, S. I. (1996). Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1327–1343. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.70.6.1327.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Ebner, N. C., Riediger, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). FACES–a database of facial expressions in young, middle-aged, and older women and men: Development and validation. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 351–362. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.1.351.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169–200. doi: 10.1080/02699939208411068.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Elliot, A. J., & Covington, M. V. (2001). Approach and avoidance motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 73–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gable, S. L. (2006). Approach and avoidance social motives and goals. Journal of Personality, 74, 175–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00373.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Gable, S. L., & Berkman, E. T. (2008). Making connections and avoiding loneliness: Approach and avoidance social motives and goals. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 204–216). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  13. Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Elliot, A. J. (2003). Evidence for bivariate systems: An empirical test of appetition and aversion across domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 349–372. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00580-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gomez, A., & Gomez, R. (2002). Personality traits of the behavioural approach and inhibition systems: Associations with processing of emotional stimuli. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1299–1316. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00119-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13, 673–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Jarvis, B. G. (2004). Direct RT (Version 2004.3.24). New York, NY: Empirisoft Corporation.Google Scholar
  17. Lee, I. A., & Preacher, K. J. (2013). Calculation for the test of the difference between two dependent correlations with one variable in common [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://quantpsy.org.
  18. McClelland, D. C. (1985). Human motivation. New York: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
  19. Mehrabian, A. (1994). Evidence bearing on the affiliative tendency (MAFF) and sensitivity to rejection (MSR) scales. Current Psychology, 13, 97–116. doi: 10.1007/BF02686794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Nikitin, J., Burgermeister, L. C., & Freund, A. M. (2012). The role of age and social motivation in developmental transitions in young and old adulthood. Frontiers in Developmental Psychology, 6, 366. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00366.Google Scholar
  21. Nikitin, J., & Freund, A. M. (2011). Age and motivation predict gaze behavior for facial expressions. Psychology and Aging, 26, 695–700. doi: 10.1037/a0023281.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Sokolowski, K., Schmalt, H. -D., Langens, T. A., & Pucca, R. M. (2000). Assessing achievement, affiliation, and power motives all at once: The multi-motive grid (MMG). Journal of Personality Assessment, 74, 126–145. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA740109.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Stanislaw, H., & Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 95382, 137–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03207704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Strachman, A., & Gable, S. L. (2006). What you want (and do not want) affects what you see (and do not see): Avoidance social goals and social events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1446–1458. doi: 10.1177/0146167206291007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.103.2.193.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of ZurichZurichSwitzerland
  2. 2.Department of Psychology and University Research Priority Program Dynamics of Healthy AgingUniversity of ZurichZurichSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations