Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 39, Issue 1, pp 119–127 | Cite as

Which emotions last longest and why: The role of event importance and rumination

Original Paper

Abstract

Some emotions last longer than others. However, duration differences have only been explored for a small number of emotions and the observed differences have not been explained. The aim of the present study is to provide a detailed picture of variability in duration between emotions and to account for this variability. Participants were asked to recollect recent emotional episodes, report their duration, and answer questions regarding appraisals and regulation strategies. Out of 27 emotions, sadness lasted the longest, whereas shame, surprise, fear, disgust, boredom, being touched, irritation, and relief were the shortest emotions. One appraisal dimension and one regulation strategy accounted for almost half of the variability in duration between emotions. In particular, compared to short emotions, persistent emotions are typically elicited by events of high importance, and are associated with high levels of rumination. This conclusion holds across emotion duration definitions, and remains valid when taking emotion recency and intensity into account.

Keywords

Emotion dynamics Emotion duration Appraisals Emotion regulation Rumination 

References

  1. Beedie, C. J., Terry, P. C., & Lane, A. M. (2005). Distinctions between emotion and mood. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 847–878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Brans, K., & Verduyn, P. (2014). Comparing determinants of emotion intensity and duration: Appraisals and regulation strategies. PLoS ONE, 9(e92410), 1–13.Google Scholar
  3. Brosschot, J. F., Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. F. (2006). The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60, 113–124.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Davidson, R. J. (1998). Affective style and affective disorders: Perspectives from affective neuroscience. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 307–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Frijda, N. H. (2007). The laws of emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  6. Frijda, N. H., Mesquita, B., Sonnemans, J., & Van Goozen, S. (1991). The duration of affective phenomena or emotions, sentiments and passions. In K. T. Strongman (Ed.), International review of studies on emotion (pp. 187–225). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  7. Gallagher, S. (2012). Time, emotion, and depression. Emotion Review, 4, 1–6.Google Scholar
  8. Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3–24). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  9. Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 121–143.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 561–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Öhman, A. (2008). Fear and anxiety: Overlaps and dissociations. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 709–729). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  13. Sbarra, D. A. (2006). Predicting the onset of emotional recovery following nonmarital relationship dissolution: Survival analyses of sadness and anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 298–312.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Scherer, K. R. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multi-level sequential checking. In K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 92–120). New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Scherer, K. R. (2005). What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Social Science Information Sur Les Sciences Sociales, 44, 695–729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Scherer, K. R., & Wallbott, H. G. (1994). Evidence for universality and cultural variation of differential emotion response patterning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 310–328.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Scherer, K. R., Wranik, T., Sangsue, J., Tran, V., & Scherer, U. (2004). Emotions in everyday life: Probability of occurrence, risk factors, appraisal and reaction pattern. Social Science Information, 43, 499–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (2000). Facets of affective experiences: A framework for investigations of trait affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 655–668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Schön, U. (2010). Recovery from severe mental illness, a gender perspective. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 24, 557–564.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Thase, M. E., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2002). Can’t shake that feeling: Event-related fMRI assessment of sustained amygdala activity in response to emotional information in depressed individuals. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 693–707.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Sonnemans, J., & Frijda, N. (1995). The determinants of subjective emotional intensity. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 483–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  23. Van Mechelen, I., Verduyn, P., & Brans, K. (2013). The duration of emotional episodes. In D. Hermans, B. Rimé, & B. Mesquita (Eds.), Changing emotions (pp. 174–180). London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  24. Verduyn, P., & Brans, K. (2012). The relationship between extraversion, neuroticism and aspects of trait affect. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 664–669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Verduyn, P., Delaveau, P., Rotgé, J. Y., Fossati, P., & Van Mechelen, I. (in press). Determinants of emotion duration and underlying psychological and neural mechanisms. Emotion Review.Google Scholar
  26. Verduyn, P., Delvaux, E., Van Coillie, H., Tuerlinckx, F., & Van Mechelen, I. (2009a). Predicting the duration of emotional experience: Two experience sampling studies. Emotion, 9, 83–91.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Verduyn, P., Van Mechelen, I., & Tuerlinckx, F. (2011). The relation between event processing and the duration of emotional experience. Emotion, 11, 20–28.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Verduyn, P., Van Mechelen, I., Tuerlinckx, F., Meers, K., & Van Coillie, H. (2009b). Intensity profiles of emotional experience over time. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1427–1443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Verduyn, P., Van Mechelen, I., Tuerlinckx, F., & Scherer, K. (2013). The relation between appraised mismatch and the duration of negative emotions: Evidence for universality. European Journal of Personality, 27, 481–494.Google Scholar
  30. Vytal, K., & Hamann, S. (2010). Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A voxel-based meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2864–2885.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Faculty of Psychology and Educational SciencesKU LeuvenLeuvenBelgium

Personalised recommendations