Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 39, Issue 1, pp 1–10 | Cite as

Enjoying the possibility of defeat: Outcome uncertainty, suspense, and intrinsic motivation

  • Sami Abuhamdeh
  • Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi
  • Baland Jalal
Original Paper

Abstract

In two studies, the relevance of outcome uncertainty and suspense for intrinsic motivation was examined. In Study 1, participants played a competitive zero-sum video game in which outcome uncertainty during the game (operationalized as the degree of parity between player–opponent scores) was manipulated. Greater outcome uncertainty led to greater enjoyment, and this effect was mediated by suspense. Although outperforming one’s opponent by a wide margin maximized perceived competence, these games were less enjoyable than closer games with higher outcome uncertainty. These findings were extended in Study 2, which incorporated a behavioral measure of intrinsic motivation. Participants chose to play games they previously rated as relatively high in suspense but relatively low in perceived competence over games which provided higher perceptions of competence but less suspense. Performance concern moderated this effect. Implications of the findings for theories of intrinsic motivation, and possible avenues for future research, are discussed.

Keywords

Outcome uncertainty Suspense Perceived competence Intrinsic motivation 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We thank Jeanne Nakamura, Timothy Rentz, and Diane Sunar for their comments on an earlier draft.

References

  1. Abuhamdeh, S., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2012). The importance of challenge for the enjoyment of intrinsically motivated, goal-directed activities. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(3), 317–330.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alwin, D. F., & Hauser, R. M. (1975). The decomposition of effects in path analysis. American Sociological Review, 40(1), 37–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Alwitt, Linda. F. (2002). Suspense and advertising responses. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(1), 35–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359–372.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brewer, W. F., & Jose, P. E. (1983). The development of story liking: Character identification, suspense, and outcome resolution. Developmental Psychology, 20(5), 911–924.Google Scholar
  6. Brewer, W. F., & Lichtenstein, E. H. (1981). Event schemas, story schemas, and story grammars. In J. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 363–379). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  7. Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  8. Carroll, N. (1996). The paradox of suspense. In P. Vorderer, H. J. Wulff, & M. Friedrichsen (Eds.), Suspense: Conceptualizations, theoretical analyses, and empirical explorations (pp. 71–91). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  9. Comisky, P., & Bryant, J. (1982). Factors involved in generating suspense. Human Communication Research, 9(1), 49–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  11. Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. (1994). Facilitating internalization: The self-determination theory perspective. Journal of Personality, 62(1), 119–142.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The ‘what’ and ‘why’ of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 227–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. (1994). Goal setting, achievement orientation, and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(5), 968–980.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Elliot, A. J., & McCregor, H. (2001). A 2 x 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(3), 501–519.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fort, R., & Quirk, J. (1995). Cross-subsidization, incentives, and outcomes in professional team sports leagues. Journal of Economic Literature, 33(3), 1265–1299.Google Scholar
  18. Frantzen, D., & Magnan, S. (2005). Anxiety and the true beginner-false beginner dynamic in beginning French and Spanish classes. Foreign Language Annals, 38(2), 171–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Freitas, A. L., & Higgins, E. T. (2002). Enjoying goal-directed action: The role of regulatory fit. Psychological Science, 13(1), 1–6.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gerrig, R. J. (1989). Suspense in the absence of uncertainty. Journal of Memory and Language, 28(6), 633–648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Guidry, J. A. (2005). The experience of…suspense: Understanding the construct, its antecedents, and ts consequences in consumption and acquisition contexts. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Texas A&M University.Google Scholar
  22. Harackiewicz, J. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). The joint effects of target and purpose goals on intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(7), 675–689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Harackiewicz, J. M., & Manderlink, G. (1984). A process analysis of the effects of performance-contingent rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 531–551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered: Toward a developmental model. Human Development, 21, 34–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hox, J. (2002). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  26. Kassler, M. (1996). Factors of suspense in narratives and film. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Memphis.Google Scholar
  27. Krane, V. (1994). The mental readiness form as a measure of competitive state anxiety. Sport Psychologist, 8(2), 189–202.Google Scholar
  28. MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 83–104.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Przybylski, A. K., Weinstein, N., Murayama, K., Lynch, M. F., & Ryan, R. M. (2012). The ideal self at play: The appeal of video games that let you be all you can be. Psychological Science, 23(1), 69–76.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Raudenbush, R. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  31. Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Elements of the competitive situation that affect intrinsic motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(1), 24–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 57–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Sansone, C. (1986). A question of competence: The effects of competence and task feedback on intrinsic interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(5), 918–931.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Sansone, C. (1989). Competence feedback, task feedback, and intrinsic interest: An examination of process and context. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25(4), 343–361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Sansone, C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). I don’t feel like it: The function of interest in self-regulation. In L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: The interaction of goals and affect. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  36. Silverman, I. (1977). The human subject in the psychological laboratory. Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  37. Snijders, T., & Bosker, R. J. (1999). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  38. Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic intervals for indirect effects in structural equations models. In S. Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 290–312). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  39. Standage, M., Duda, J. L., & Pensgaard, A. M. (2005). The effect of competitive outcome and task-involving, ego-involving, and cooperative structures on the psychological well-being of individuals engaged in a co-ordination task: A self-determination approach. Motivation and Emotion, 29(1), 41–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Vallerand, R. J., Gauvin, L. I., & Halliwell, W. R. (1986). Effects of zero-sum competition on children’s intrinsic motivation and perceived competence. Journal of Social Psychology, 126(4), 465–472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Vansteenkiste, M., & Deci, E. L. (2003). Competitively contingent rewards and intrinsic motivation: Can losers remain motivated? Motivation & Emotion, 27(4), 273–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Vorderer, P., Wulff, H.J., & Friedrichsen, M. (Eds.). (1996). Suspense: Conceptualizations, theoretical analyses, and empirical explorations. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  43. Weinberg, R. S., & Ragan, J. (1979). Effects of competition, success/failure, and sex on intrinsic motivation. Research Quarterly, 50, 503–510.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Sami Abuhamdeh
    • 1
  • Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi
    • 2
  • Baland Jalal
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyIstanbul Şehir UniversityIstanbulTurkey
  2. 2.Claremont Graduate UniversityClaremontUSA
  3. 3.University of California, San DiegoLa JollaUSA

Personalised recommendations