Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 36, Issue 2, pp 181–194 | Cite as

On boredom: Lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences

Original Paper

Abstract

Boredom is a common experience that affects people on multiple levels, including their thoughts, feelings, motivations, and actions. Not much research, however, has examined what makes the experience of boredom distinct from other affective experiences. Based on earlier research on boredom and our meaning-regulation framework, we conducted a series of four studies that demonstrate the distinct experiential content of boredom. More than other negative affective experiences (sadness, anger, and frustration), boredom makes people feel unchallenged while they think that the situation and their actions are meaningless (Study 1). The distinct experiential content of boredom is associated with boredom proneness (Study 2) and with state boredom experiences (Study 3). In addition, the distinct experiential content of boredom is affected by contextual features (Study 4). This series of studies provides a systematic understanding of what people feel, think, and want to do when bored, distinctive from other negative experiences.

Keywords

Boredom Challenge Meaning Emotion 

References

  1. Acee, T. W., Kim, H., Kim, H. J., Kim, J. I., Chu, H. R., Kim, M., et al. (2010). Academic boredom in under- and over-challenging situations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 17–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barbalet, J. M. (1999). Boredom and social meaning. British Journal of Sociology, 50, 631–646.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bargdill, R. W. (2000). The study of life boredom. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 31, 188–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blaszczynski, A., McConaghy, N., & Frankova, A. (1990). Boredom proneness in pathological gambling. Psychological Reports, 67, 35–42.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Clore, G. L., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2007). Affect-as-information. In R. Baumeister & K. Vohs (Eds.), Encyclopedia of social psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  6. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Beyond boredom and anxiety: Experiencing flow in work and play. San Fransisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  7. Dahlen, E. R., Martin, R. C., Ragan, K., & Kuhlman, M. M. (2004). Boredom proneness in anger and aggression: Effects of impulsiveness and sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1615–1627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Eastwood, J. D., Cavaliere, C., Fahlman, S. A., & Eastwood, A. E. (2007). A desire for desires: Boredom and its relation to alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1035–1045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fahlman, S. A., Mercer, K. B., Gaskovski, P., Eastwood, A. E., & Eastwood, J. D. (2009). Does a lack of meaning cause boredom? Results from psychometric, longitudinal, and experimental analyses. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 307–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Farmer, R., & Sundberg, N. D. (1986). Boredom proneness—the development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 4–17.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Fisher, C. D. (1998). Effects of external and internal interruptions on boredom at work: Two studies. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 503–522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43, 349–358.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Frijda, N. H. (2007). The laws of emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  15. Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & Ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fromm, E. (1973). The anatomy of human destructiveness. New York: Holt McDougal.Google Scholar
  17. Gordon, A., Wilkinson, R., McGrown, A., & Jovanoska, S. (1997). The psychometric properties of the boredom proneness scale: An examination of its validity. Psychological Studies, 42, 85–97.Google Scholar
  18. Greenberg, J., Koole, S. L., & Pyszczynski, T. (2004). Handbook of experimental existential psychology. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  19. Harris, J. J., & Segal, D. R. (1985). Observations from the Sinai: The boredom factor. Armed Forces and Society, 37, 235–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis is the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Heine, S. J., Proulx, T., & Vohs, K. D. (2006). The meaning maintenance model: On the coherence of social motivations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 88–110.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kanevsky, L., & Keighley, T. (2003). To produce or not to produce? Understanding boredom and the honor in underachievement. Roeper Review, 26, 20–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kass, S. J., Vadanovich, S. J., & Callender, A. (2001). State-trait boredom: Relationship to absenteeism, tenure, and job satisfaction. Journal of Business and Psychology, 16, 317–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Klapp, O. E. (1986). Overload and boredom: Essays on the quality of life in the information society. New York: Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
  25. Lazarus, R. S., & Smith, C. A. (1988). Knowledge and appraisal in the cognition-emotion relationship. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 281–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Leary, M. R., Rogers, P. A., Canfield, R. W., & Coe, C. (1986). Boredom in interpersonal encounters: Antecedents and social implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 958–975.Google Scholar
  27. Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., & Hall, N. C. (2011). Coping with boredom in school: An experience sampling perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 49–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., & Perry, R. P. (2010). Boredom in achievement settings: Control-value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 531–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavioral Research Methods, 40, 879–891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, and goal differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 206–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Rupp, D. E., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1997). The role of boredom proneness in self-reported anger and aggression. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 925–936.Google Scholar
  32. Sansone, C., Weir, C., Harpster, L., & Morgan, C. (1992). Once a boring task always a boring task? Interest as a self-regulatory strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 379–390.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Scherer, K. R. (1997). The role of culture in emotion-antecedent appraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 902–922.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology. A handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 385–407). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  36. Silvia, P. J. (2006). Exploring the psychology of interest. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Smith, J. L., Wagaman, J., & Handley, I. M. (2009). Keeping it dull or making it fun: Task variation as a function of promotion versus prevention focus. Motivation and Emotion, 33, 150–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Stickney, M. L., & Miltenberger, R. G. (1999). Evaluating direct and indirect measures for the functional assessment of binge eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 195–204.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  41. Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Ciarocco, N. J., & Bartels, J. M. (2007). Social exclusion decreases prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 56–66.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Van de Ven, N., Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. (2009). Leveling up and down: The experiences of benign and malicious envy. Emotion, 9, 419–429.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Van Tilburg, W. A. P., & Igou, E. R. (2011). Why bored George helps others; A pragmatic meaning-regulation hypothesis on boredom and prosocial behavior. Under Review. Google Scholar
  44. Van Tilburg, W. A. P., & Igou, E. R. (in press). On boredom and social identity: A pragmatic meaning-regulation approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.Google Scholar
  45. Vodanovich, S. J. (2003). Psychometric properties of boredom: A review of the literature. Journal of Psychology, 137, 569–595.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Vodanovich, S. J., & Kass, S. J. (1990). A factor analytic study of the boredom proneness scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 115–123.Google Scholar
  47. Vodanovich, S. J., Verner, K. M., & Gilbride, T. V. (1991). Boredom proneness: Its relationship to positive and negative affect. Psychological Reports, 69, 1139–1146.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Wallbott, H. G. (1998). Bodily expression of emotion. European Journal of Psychology, 28, 879–896.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Computer Science BuildingUniversity of LimerickCastletroyRepublic of Ireland

Personalised recommendations