Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 29, Issue 3, pp 165–188 | Cite as

When Effort Is Enjoyed: Two Studies of Intrinsic Motivation for Personally Salient Activities

  • Alan S. WatermanEmail author
Article

Abstract

Distinctions between two philosophical conceptions of happiness, hedonism and eudaimonism, were applied to the study of intrinsic motivation. Modified versions of the Personally Expressive Activities Questionnaire (PEAQ) were used in two studies to contrast activities, all of which were enjoyed, but which differed in the level of effort involved. In Study 1, 173 college students were free to choose any type of activity that met the selection criteria. In Study 2, the activities chosen by 95 undergraduates were limited to activities associated with a particular leisure time or hobby activity in which the respondents engaged on a regular basis. Consistent results across the two studies indicate that High Effort–Liked activities, in comparison to Low Effort–Liked activities, were associated with greater interest, flow, and feelings of personal expressiveness, greater perceived competence, and higher scores for both self-realization values and importance. These differences are discussed for their implications for the conceptual understanding of intrinsic motivation.

Keywords

intrinsic motivation effort flow self-determination self-realization 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ackrill, J. L. (1973). Aristotle's ethics. London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
  2. Aristotle. (1985). Nichomachean ethics (T. Irwin, Trans.). Indianapolis, IN: Hackett.Google Scholar
  3. Assor, A., Kaplan, H., & Roth, G. (2002). Choice is good, but relevance is excellent: Autonomy enhancing and suppressing teacher behaviours predicting students' engagement in schoolwork. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 261–278.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bentham, J. (1962). An introduction to the principles of morals and legislation. In J. Bowring (Ed.). The works of Jeremy Bentham. New York: Russell & Russell. (Original work published 1789).Google Scholar
  5. Boggiano, A. K., Main, D., & Katz, P. A. (1988). Children's preference for challenge: The role of perceived competence and control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 134–141.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Carver, C. S., & Baird, E. (1998). The American dream revisited: Is it what you want or why you want it that matters? Psychological Science, 9, 289–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  8. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). The flow experience and its significance for human psychology. In M. Csikszentmihaly & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.). Opitmal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 15–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  10. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding flow: The psychology of engagement in everyday life. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  11. Curry, F., Biddle, S., Famose, J.-P., & Goudas, M. (1996). Personality and situational factors influencing intrinsic interest of adolescent girls in school physical education: A structural equation modeling analysis. Educational Psychololgy, 16, 305–315.Google Scholar
  12. de Spinoza, B. (1951). The ethics. In The chief works of Benedict de Spinoza (R. H. M. Elwes, Trans.). New York: Dover: (Original work published 1677).Google Scholar
  13. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  14. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (Eds.). (2002). Handbook of self-determination research. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
  15. Dwyer, J. M. (1993). Effect of perceived choice of music on exercise intrinsic motivation. Health Values: The Journal of Health Behavior, Education, and Promotion, 19(2), 18–26.Google Scholar
  16. Elliot, A. J., Faler, J., McGregor, H. A., Campbell, W. K., Sedikides, C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Competence valuation as a strategic intrinsic motivation process. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 780–794.Google Scholar
  17. Emmons, R. A. (1999). The psychology of ultimate concerns: Motivation and spirituality in personality. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  18. Felixbrod, J. J., & O'Leary, K. D. (1973). Effects of reinforcement on children's academic behavior as a function of self-determined and externally imposed contingencies. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6, 241–250.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Fromm, E. (1947). Man for himself: An inquiry into the psychology of ethics. New York: Fawcett.Google Scholar
  20. Graef, R., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Giannino, S. M. (1983). Measuring intrinsic motivation in everyday life. Leisure Studies, 2, 155–168.Google Scholar
  21. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410–422.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: Well-being correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 281–288.Google Scholar
  23. Krapp, A., Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (1992). Interest, learning, and development. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.). The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 3–26). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  24. Kraut, R. (1979). Two conceptions of happiness. Philosophical Review, 87, 167–196.Google Scholar
  25. LaMore, K., & Nelson, D. L. (1993). The effect of options on performance of an art project in adults with mental disabilities. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 47, 397–401.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Lee, R., & Graham, W. K. (1986). Self-actualization need strength: Moderator of relationships between job characteristics and job outcomes. Journal of Employment Counseling, 23, 38–47.Google Scholar
  27. Lee, R., McCabe, D. J., & Graham, W. K. (1983). Multivariate relationship between job characteristics and job satisfaction in the public sector: A triple cross-validation study. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 18, 47–62.Google Scholar
  28. Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd ed.). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
  29. McDowell, J. (1980). The role of eudaimonia in Aristotle's ethics. In A. O. Rorty (Ed.). Essays on Aristotle's ethics (pp. 359–376). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  30. Norton, D. L. (1976). Personal destinies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Plato. (1937). The dialogues of Plato (B. Jowett, Trans.). New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  32. Pryor, S. K. (1999). The effect of contingency contracting on enhancing intrinsic motivation and exercise behavior in ten and eleven year old girls. Dissertation Abstracts, 59, 5313.Google Scholar
  33. Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Elements of the competitive situation that affect intrinsic motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 24–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Ryan, R. M. (1993). Agency and organization: Intrinsic motivation, autonomy and the self in psychological development. In J. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Developmental perspectives on motivation (Vol. 40, pp. 1–56). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  35. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Sansone, C., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). “I don't feel like it”: The function of interest in self-regulation. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.). Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 203–228). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  38. Schwartz, S. J., & Waterman, A. S. (2005). Changing interests: A longitudinal study of intrinsic motivation for personally salient activities. Manuscript under editorial review.Google Scholar
  39. Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). Not all personal goals are personal: Comparing autonomous and controlled reasons for goals as predictors of effort and attainment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 546–557.Google Scholar
  40. Tatarkiewicz, W. (1976). Analysis of happiness. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
  41. Telfer, E. (1980). Happiness. NewYork: St. Martin's Press.Google Scholar
  42. Vallerand, R. J., & Ratelle, C. F. (2002). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: A hierarchical model. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.). Handbook of self-determination research. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
  43. Vallerand, R. J., & Reid, G. (1984). On the causal effects of perceived competence on intrinsic motivation: A test of cognitive-evaluation theory. Journal of Sport Psychology, 6, 94–102.Google Scholar
  44. Waterman, A. S. (1990). Personal expressiveness: Philosophical and psychological foundations. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 11, 47–74.Google Scholar
  45. Waterman, A. S. (1992). Identity as an aspect of optimal psychological functioning. In G. R. Adams, T. Gullotta, & R. Montemayor (Eds.), Advances in adolescent development: Vol. 4. Identity formation during adolescence (pp. 50–72). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  46. Waterman, A. S. (1993a). Finding something to do or someone to be: A eudaemonist perspective on identity formation. In J. Kroger (Ed.). Discussions on ego identity (pp. 147–167). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  47. Waterman, A. S. (1993b). Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 678–691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Waterman, A. S. (1998). The Personally Expressive Activities Questionnaire: A manual. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  49. Waterman, A. S., Schwartz, S. J., & Conti, R. (2005). The implications of two conceptions of happiness (hedonic enjoyment and eudaimonia) for the understanding of intrinsic motivation. Manuscript under editorial review.Google Scholar
  50. Waterman, A. S., Schwartz, S. J., Goldbacher, E., Green, H., Miller, C., & Philip, S. (2003). Predicting the subjective experience of intrinsic motivation: The roles of self-determination, the balance of challenges and skills, and self-realization values. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1447–1458.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Zuckerman, M., Porac, J., Lathin, D., Smith, R., & Deci, E. L. (1978). On the importance of self-determination for intrinsically motivated behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 443–446.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science + Business Media, Inc. 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyThe College of New JerseyEwing
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyThe College of New JerseyEwing

Personalised recommendations