Minds and Machines

, Volume 20, Issue 3, pp 363–383 | Cite as

A View on Human Goal-Directed Activity and the Construction of Artificial Intelligence

Article

Abstract

Although activity aimed at the construction of artificial intelligence started about 60 years ago however, contemporary intelligent systems are effective in very narrow domains only. One of the reasons for this situation appears to be serious problems in the theory of intelligence. Intelligence is a characteristic of goal-directed systems and two classes of goal-directed systems can be derived from observations on animals and humans, one class is systems with innately and jointly determined goals and means. The other class contains systems that are able to construct arbitrary goals and means. It is suggested that the classes (that implicitly underlie most models of artificial intelligence) are insufficient to explain human goal-directed activity. A broader approach to goal-directed systems is considered. This approach suggests that humans are goal-directed systems that jointly synthesize arbitrary goals and means. Neural and psychological data favoring this hypothesis and its experimental validation are considered. A simple computer model based on the idea of joint synthesis to simulate goal-directed activity is presented. The usage of the idea of joint synthesis for the construction of artificial intelligence is discussed.

Keywords

Goal-directed activity Intelligence Synthesis Self-organization 

References

  1. Asaad, W. F., Rainer, G., & Miller, E. K. (2000). Task-specific neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 84, 451–459.Google Scholar
  2. Bäck, T. (1996). Evolutionary algorithms in theory and practice: Evolution strategies, evolutionary programming, genetic algorithms. Oxford: Oxford University Press.MATHGoogle Scholar
  3. Batuev, A. S. (1981). High integrative systems of the brain. Moscow: Nauka. (in Russian).Google Scholar
  4. Bertino, E., Piero, G., & Zarria, B. C. (2001). Intelligent database systems. Reading: Addison-Wesley Professional.Google Scholar
  5. Buller, D. J. (1998) DeFreuding evolutionary psychology: Adaptation and human motivation. In Hardcastle, V. G. (Ed.), Psychology meets biology: Connections, conjectures, constraints. MIT Press/Bradford Books. http://cogprints.soton.ac.uk/documents/disk0/00/00/03/26/cog00000326-00/defreud.htm.
  6. Cooper, R. P., & Shallice, T. (1995). Soar and the case for unified theories of cognition. Cognition, 55(2), 115–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Corkill, D. D. (1991). Blackboard systems. AI Expert, 6(9), 40–47.Google Scholar
  8. Corsini, R. J., & Auerbach, A. J. (1998). Concise encyclopedia of psychology. Wiley.Google Scholar
  9. Craig, I. (1995). Blackboard systems (Tutorial monographs in artificial intelligence series). Ablex Publishing Corporation.Google Scholar
  10. Evans, J. S. B. T. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 454–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Fuster, J. M. (1997). Network memory. Trends Neuroscience, 20, 451–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Haykin, S. (1998). Neural networks: A comprehensive foundation. Prentice Hall, Englewood cliffs.Google Scholar
  13. Heckhausen, H. (1980). Motivation und Handeln. Verlag: Springer.Google Scholar
  14. Holland, J. H. (1975). Adaptation in natural and artificial systems. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
  15. Jackson, P. (1998). Introduction to expert systems (3rd ed.). Addison Wesley.Google Scholar
  16. Joseph, R. (1999). Frontal lobe psychopathology: Mania, depression, confabulation, catatonia, perseveration, obsessive compulsions, and schizophrenia. Psychiatry, 62(2), 138–172.Google Scholar
  17. Leslie, K. P., Littman, M. L., & Moore, A. W. (1996). Reinforcement learning: A survey. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research., 4, 237–285.Google Scholar
  18. Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. Andover, Hants: Tavistock. Publications.Google Scholar
  19. Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Miller, B. T., & D’Esposito, M. (2005). Searching for “the top” in top-down control. Neuron, 48, 535–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard.Google Scholar
  22. Rodina, O. N., & Prudkov, P. N. (2005). The principle of joint synthesis in purposeful processes. MGU Bulletin Psychology, 2, 77–86. (in Russian).Google Scholar
  23. Russell, S. J., & Norvig, P. (2003). Artificial intelligence: A modern approach. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  24. Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Sutton, R. S. & Barto, A.G. (1998). Reinforcement learning: An introduction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  26. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., & Barrett, H. C. (2005). Resolving the debate on innate ideas: Learnability constraints and the evolved interpenetration of motivational and conceptual functions. In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), The innate mind: Structure and content. NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Van der Velde, F., & de Kamps, M. (2003). A model of visual working memory in PFC. Neurocomputing, 52–54, 419–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Van der Velde, F., & de Kamps, M. (2006). Neural blackboard architectures of combinatorial structures in cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 37–70.Google Scholar
  30. von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General systems theory: Foundation, development, applications. New York: Brazillier.Google Scholar
  31. Wood, J. N., & Grafman, J. (2003). Human prefrontal cortex: Processing and representational perspectives. Nature Review Neuroscience, 4, 139–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Ecomon Ltd.MoscowRussia

Personalised recommendations