Advertisement

Marketing Letters

, Volume 29, Issue 4, pp 435–449 | Cite as

Are large portions always bad? Using the Delboeuf illusion on food packaging to nudge consumer behavior

  • Olivia PetitEmail author
  • Carlos Velasco
  • Charles Spence
Article

Abstract

Exaggerated portion sizes are generally pictured on the front of product packaging in order to stimulate food craving and encourage consumer purchasing decisions. However, one problem with such images is that they can set inappropriate norms as far as food consumption is concerned and hence result in people serving themselves more than they otherwise might. The research reported here builds on the fact that depicting a food portion in a smaller (vs. larger) container (i.e., plate or bowl) creates the illusion of a larger (vs. smaller) portion, although the actual quantity of food remains the same (this is known as the Delboeuf illusion). Here, we demonstrate in two experiments that by presenting food in a smaller container (thus giving rise to the illusion of a relatively larger portion), participants have higher purchase intentions (study 1) and perceive the food as being more appetizing (study 2) but, crucially, decrease the size of the portion that they serve themselves (studies 1 and 2). Overall, by giving the impression of a larger portion on product packaging, the Delboeuf illusion could potentially be used to nudge consumers to find food more desirable, while at the same time leading them to reduce their serving, thus potentially benefitting both consumers and the food industry.

Keywords

Food packaging Portion size Visual illusions Mental imagery Embodied self-regulation Nudge 

Notes

References

  1. Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J. F., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5(6), 679–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Akyol, A., Ayaz, A., Inan-Eroglu, E., Cetin, C., & Samur, G. (2018). Impact of three different plate colours on short-term satiety and energy intake: a randomized controlled trial. Nutrition Journal, 17(1), 46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Basso, F., Petit, O., Le Bellu, S., Lahlou, S., Cancel, A., & Anton, J. L. (2018). Taste at first (person) sight: visual perspective modulates brain activity implicitly associated with viewing unhealthy but not healthy foods. Appetite, 128, 242–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brand, J., & Wansink, B. (2016). Depicted versus stated serving sizes: exaggerated servings on packaging encourage overeating. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 48(7), S91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brunstrom, J. M., & Shakeshaft, N. G. (2009). Measuring affective (liking) and non-affective (expected satiety) determinants of portion size and food reward. Appetite, 52(1), 108–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cornil, Y., & Chandon, P. (2016). Pleasure as a substitute for size: how multisensory imagery can make people happier with smaller food portions. Journal of Marketing Research, 53(5), 847–864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Delboeuf, F. J. (1865). Note sur certaines illusions d’optique: essai d’une théorie psychophysique de la manière dont l’œil apprécie les distances et les angles [Note on certain optical illusions: essay on a psychophysical theory concerning the way in which the eye evaluates distances and angles]. Bulletins de l’Académie Royale des Sciences, Lettres et Beaux-arts de Belgique, 19, 2nd ser., pp 195–216.Google Scholar
  9. Elder, R. S., & Krishna, A. (2012). The “visual depiction effect” in advertising: facilitating embodied mental simulation through product orientation. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(6), 988–1003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Franz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: no evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11(1), 20–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Guthrie, J., Mancino, L., & Lin, C. T. J. (2015). Nudging consumers toward better food choices: policy approaches to changing food consumption behaviors. Psychology and Marketing, 32(5), 501–511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Holden, S. S., Zlatevska, N., & Dubelaar, C. (2016). Whether smaller plates reduce consumption depends on who’s serving and who’s looking: a meta-analysis. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 1(1), 134–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Labbe, D., Rytz, A., Godinot, N., Ferrage, A., & Martin, N. (2017). Is portion size selection associated with expected satiation, perceived healthfulness or expected tastiness? A case study on pizza using a photograph-based computer task. Appetite, 108, 311–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Madzharov, A. V., & Block, L. G. (2010). Effects of product unit image on consumption of snack foods. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20(4), 398–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. McClain, A. D., van den Bos, W., Matheson, D., Desai, M., McClure, S. M., & Robinson, T. N. (2014). Visual illusions and plate design: the effects of plate rim widths and rim coloring on perceived food portion size. International Journal of Obesity, 38(5), 657–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Papies, E. K. (2013). Tempting food words activate eating simulations. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Peng, M., Adam, S., Hautus, M. J., Shin, M., Duizer, L. M., & Yan, H. (2017). See food diet? Cultural differences in estimating fullness and intake as a function of plate size. Appetite, 117, 197–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Petit, O., Basso, F., Merunka, D., Spence, C., Cheok, A. D., & Oullier, O. (2016a). Pleasure and the control of food intake: an embodied cognition approach to consumer self-regulation. Psychology and Marketing, 33(8), 608–619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Petit, O., Merunka, D., Anton, J.-L., Nazarian, B., Spence, C., Cheok, A. D., Raccah, D., & Oullier, O. (2016b). Health and pleasure in consumers’ dietary food choices: individual differences in the brain’s value system. PLoS One, 11(7), e0156333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Petit, O., Spence, C., Velasco, C., Woods, A. T., & Cheok, A. D. (2017). Changing the influence of portion size on consumer behavior via imagined consumption. Journal of Business Research, 75, 240–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Robinson, E., Nolan, S., Tudur-Smith, C., Boyland, E. J., Harrold, J. A., Hardman, C. A., & Halford, J. C. (2014). Will smaller plates lead to smaller waists? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect that experimental manipulation of dishware size has on energy consumption. Obesity Reviews, 15(10), 812–821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Sawada, R., Sato, W., Toichi, M., & Fushiki, T. (2017). Fat content modulates rapid detection of food: a visual search study using fast food and Japanese diet. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1033.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Spence, C., Okajima, K., Cheok, A. D., Petit, O., & Michel, C. (2016). Eating with our eyes: from visual hunger to digital satiation. Brain and Cognition, 110, 53–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Tal, A., Niemann, S., & Wansink, B. (2017). Depicted serving size: cereal packaging pictures exaggerate serving sizes and promote overserving. BMC Public Health, 17(1), 169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. (2008). Nudge: improving decisions about health, wealth and happiness. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
  26. Toepel, U., Bielser, M. L., Forde, C., Martin, N., Voirin, A., le Coutre, J., Murray, M. M., & Hudry, J. (2015). Brain dynamics of meal size selection in humans. NeuroImage, 113, 133–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Van der Laan, L. N., De Ridder, D. T. D., Viergever, M. A., & Smeets, P. A. (2011). The first taste is always with the eyes: a meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. NeuroImage, 55(1), 296–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Van Ittersum, K., & Wansink, B. (2012). Plate size and color suggestibility: the Delboeuf Illusion’s bias on serving and eating behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(2), 215–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Velasco, C., & Spence, C. (Eds.). (2019). Multisensory packaging: designing new product experiences. Cham: Palgrave MacMillan.Google Scholar
  30. Versluis, I., & Papies, E. K. (2016). The role of social norms in the portion size effect: reducing normative relevance reduces the effect of portion size on consumption decisions. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Wansink, B., Painter, J. E., & North, J. (2005). Bottomless bowls: why visual cues of portion size may influence intake. Obesity Research, 13(1), 93–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Xie, H., Minton, E. A., & Kahle, L. R. (2016). Cake or fruit? Influencing healthy food choice through the interaction of automatic and instructed mental simulation. Marketing Letters, 27(4), 627–644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of MarketingKEDGE Business SchoolMarseilleFrance
  2. 2.Centre for Multisensory Marketing, Department of MarketingBI Norwegian Business SchoolOsloNorway
  3. 3.Crossmodal Research LaboratoryDepartment of Experimental PsychologyOxfordUK

Personalised recommendations