Marketing Letters

, Volume 21, Issue 1, pp 17–35 | Cite as

I’ll have the ice cream soon and the vegetables later: A study of online grocery purchases and order lead time

  • Katherine L. Milkman
  • Todd Rogers
  • Max H. Bazerman
Article

Abstract

How do decisions made for tomorrow or 2 days in the future differ from decisions made for several days in the future? We use data from an online grocer to address this question. In general, we find that as the delay between order completion and delivery increases, grocery customers spend less, order a higher percentage of “should” items (e.g., vegetables), and order a lower percentage of “want” items (e.g., ice cream), controlling for customer fixed effects. These field results replicate previous laboratory findings and are consistent with theories suggesting that people’s should selves exert more influence over their choices the further in the future outcomes will be experienced. However, orders placed for delivery tomorrow versus 2 days in the future do not show this want/should pattern, and we discuss a potential explanation.

Keywords

Lead time Intertemporal choice Want/should E-commerce Intrapersonal conflict 

References

  1. Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 463–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ashraf, N., Karlan, D., & Yin, W. (2006). Tying Odysseus to the mast: Evidence from a commitment savings product in the Philippines. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121(2), 635–672.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bazerman, M. H., Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Wade-Benzoni, K. (1998). Negotiating with yourself and losing: Making decisions with competing internal preferences. Academy of Management Review, 23(2), 225–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Benzion, U., Rapopport, A., & Yagil, Y. (1989). Discount rates inferred from decisions: An experimental study. Management Science, 35(3), 270–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. comScore Press Release (2007) comScore Networks reports total non-travel e-commerce spending reaches $102 billion in 2006; up 24 percent versus 2005. comScore Press Release. January 3, 2007. Accessed April 30, 2008: http://www.comscore.com/press/release.asp?press=1166.
  6. Drewnowski, A., & Barratt-Fornell, A. (2004). Do healthier diets cost more? Nutrition Today, 39(4), 161–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Heilman, C. M., Nakamoto, K., & Rao, A. G. (2002). Pleasant surprises: Consumer response to unexpected in-store coupons. Journal of Marketing Research, 39(2), 242–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Khan, U., Dhar, R., & Wertenbroch, K. (2005). A behavioral decision theory perspective on hedonic and utilitarian choice. In S. Ratneshwar & D. G. Mick (Eds.), Inside consumption: Frontiers of research on consumer motives, goals, and desires (pp. 144–165). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  9. Loewenstein, G. F. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65(3), 272–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Loewenstein, G. F., & Prelec, D. (1992). Anomalies in intertemporal choice: Evidence and an interpretation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107, 573–597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Malmendier, U., & Della Vigna, S. (2006). Paying not to go to the gym. American Economic Review, 96(3), 694–719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Martin, J. M., Beshears, J., Milkman, K. L., Bazerman, M. H., & Sutherland, L. (2009). Modeling expert opinions on food healthiness: A nutrition metric. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 109(6), 1088–1091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Milkman, K. L., & Beshears, J. (2009). Mental accounting and small windfalls: Evidence from an online grocer. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 71(2), 384–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Milkman, K. L., Rogers, T., & Bazerman, M. H. (2008). Harnessing our inner angels and demons: What we have learned about want/should conflicts and how that knowledge can help us reduce short-sighted decision making. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 324–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Milkman, K. L., Rogers, T., & Bazerman, M. H. (2009). Highbrow films gather dust: Time-inconsistent preferences and online DVD rentals. Management Science, 55(6), 1047–1059.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Oster, S., & Scott Morton, F. M. (2005). Behavioral biases meet the market: The case of magazine subscription prices. BE Journals Economic Analysis and Policy—Advances, 5(1), 1323.Google Scholar
  17. Read, D. (2001). Intrapersonal dilemmas. Human Relations, 54(8), 1093–1117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Rogers, T., & Bazerman, M. H. (2008). Future lock-in: Future implementation increases selection of should choices. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 106(1), 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Schelling, T. C. (1984). Choice and consequence: Perspectives of an errant economist. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Shefrin, H., & Thaler, R. H. (1988). The behavioral life-cycle hypothesis. Economic Inquiry, 26(4), 609–643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86(2), 420–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Sunstein, C. R., & Thaler, R. H. (2003). Libertarian paternalism is not an oxymoron. University of Chicago Law Review, 70, 1159–1199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110, 403–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Van Duyn, M. A. S., & Pivonka, E. (2000). Overview of the health benefits of fruit and vegetable consumption for the dietetics profession: Selected literature. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 100(12), 1511–1521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Willet, W. C. (1994). Diet and health: What should we eat? Science, 264(5158), 532–537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Wooldridge, J. M. (2002). Econometric analysis of cross section and panel data. Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  27. Zauberman, G., & Lynch, J. G. (2005). Resource slack and propensity to discount delayed investments of time versus money. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(1), 23–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Katherine L. Milkman
    • 1
  • Todd Rogers
    • 2
  • Max H. Bazerman
    • 3
  1. 1.The Wharton SchoolPhiladelphiaUSA
  2. 2.Analyst InstituteWashingtonUSA
  3. 3.Harvard Business SchoolBostonUSA

Personalised recommendations