Advertisement

Journal of Science Education and Technology

, Volume 23, Issue 1, pp 26–36 | Cite as

Scientific Investigations of Elementary School Children

  • Nicos Valanides
  • Maria Papageorgiou
  • Charoula AngeliEmail author
Article

Abstract

The study provides evidence concerning elementary school children’s ability to conduct a scientific investigation. Two hundred and fifty sixth-grade students and 248 fourth-grade students were administered a test, and based on their performance, they were classified into high-ability and low-ability students. The sample of this study was randomly selected and included 80 students, 40 fourth-grade and 40 sixth-grade students of low and high abilities. Students were specifically instructed to investigate the functioning of a device, to think aloud prior and after any experiment with the device, and to keep a record of their experimental results. The results showed that students were inclined to mainly collect evidence from the experimental space and failed to control variables during their investigation. The majority of the students had difficulties with effectively organizing collected data and failed to coordinate hypotheses with evidence. The significant interaction effect that was found between grade level and ability in terms of students’ investigation ability indicates that the existing gap between high- and low-ability students becomes bigger as students become older. Undoubtedly, ongoing research efforts for identifying patterns of children’s cognitive development will be most valuable as they can have important implications for the design of teaching scenarios and inquiry-based science activities conducive to accelerating students’ cognitive growth and scientific investigation abilities.

Keywords

Scientific investigations Scientific reasoning Experimentation Science education Fair test Variables 

References

  1. Adey P (2005) Issues arising from the long-term evaluation of cognitive accelerating programs. Res Sci Educ 35:3–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Brunken L, Plass JL, Leutner D (2003) Direct measurement of cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educ Psychol 38:53–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Germann PJ, Aram RJ, Burke G (1996) Identifying patterns and relationships among the responses of seventh-grade students to the science process skill of designing experiments. J Res Sci Teach 33:79–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Glaser BG, Strauss AL (1967) The discovery of grounded theory: strategies for qualitative research. Aldine Publications, Chicago, ILGoogle Scholar
  5. Gleason ME, Schauble L (2000) Parents’ assistance of their children’s scientific reasoning. Cogn Instr 17:343–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Gott R, Duggan S (1995) Investigative work in the science curriculum. Open University Press, BuckinghamGoogle Scholar
  7. Keselman A (2003) Supporting inquiry learning by promoting normative understanding of multivariate causality. J Res Sci Teach 40:898–921CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Keys CW (1998) A study of grade six students generating questions and plans for open-ended science investigations. Res Sci Educ 28:301–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Klahr D (2000) Exploring science: the cognition and development of discovery processes. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  10. Klahr D, Dunbar K (1988) Dual space search during scientific reasoning. Cogn Sci 12:1–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Klahr D, Dunbar K, Fay A (2000) Developmental aspects of scientific reasoning. In: Klahr D (ed) Exploring science: the cognition and development of discovery processes. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  12. Klayman J, Ha YW (1987) Confirmation, disconfirmation and information in hypothesis testing. Psychol Rev 94:211–228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Kuhn D (1989) Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychol Rev 96:674–689CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kuhn D, Pearsall S (2000) Developmental origins of scientific thinking. J Cogn Dev 1:113–129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kuhn D, Garcia-Mila M, Zohar A, Andersen C (1995) Strategies of knowledge acquisition. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 60(40):1–128Google Scholar
  16. Liu X, Lesniak KM (2005) Students’ progression of understanding the matter concept from elementary to high school. Sci Educ 89:433–450CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. McCloskey M (1983) Naïve theories of motion. In: Gentner D, Stevenns AL (eds) Mental models. Erlbaum Lawrence Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 299–334Google Scholar
  18. Mynatt CR, Doherty ME, Tweney RD (1978) Consequences of confirmation and disconfirmation in a simulated research environment. Q J Exp Psychol 30:395–406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Nakhleh MB, Samarapungavan A, Saglam Y (2005) Middle school students’ beliefs about matter. J Res Sci Teach 42:581–612CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Piaget J, Inhelder B (1974) The child’s construction of quantities: conservation and atomism. Routledge & Kegan Paul, LondonGoogle Scholar
  21. Schauble L (1990) Belief revision in children: the role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. J Exp Child Psychol 49:31–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Schauble L (1996) The development of scientific reasoning in knowledge-rich contexts. Dev Psychol 32:102–119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Schauble L, Klopher LE, Raghavan K (1991) Students’ transition from an engineering model to a science model of experimentation. J Res Sci Teach 28:859–882CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Schauble L, Glaser R, Duschl RA, Schulze S, John J (1995) Students’ understanding of the objectives and procedures of experimentation in the science classroom. J Learn Sci 4:131–166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Sharp JG, Kuerbis P (2006) Children’s ideas about the solar system and the chaos in learning science. Sci Educ 90:124–147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Strauss AL, Corbin J (1990) Basics of qualitative research: grounded theory procedures and techniques. Sage, Newbury Park, CAGoogle Scholar
  27. Sweller J, van Merrienboer JJG, Paas F (1998) Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educ Psychol Rev 10:251–296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Trafton JG, Trickett SB (2001) Note-taking for self-explanation and problem solving. Hum Comput Interact 16:1–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Zimmerman C (2000) The development of scientific reasoning skills. Dev Rev 20:99–149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Zimmerman C (2007) The development of scientific thinking skills in elementary and middle school. Dev Rev 27:172–223CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Nicos Valanides
    • 1
  • Maria Papageorgiou
    • 1
  • Charoula Angeli
    • 1
    Email author
  1. 1.Department of EducationUniversity of CyprusNicosiaCyprus

Personalised recommendations