Journal of Science Education and Technology

, Volume 18, Issue 4, pp 334–352 | Cite as

Teachable Agents and the Protégé Effect: Increasing the Effort Towards Learning

  • Catherine C. Chase
  • Doris B. Chin
  • Marily A. Oppezzo
  • Daniel L. Schwartz
Article

Abstract

Betty’s Brain is a computer-based learning environment that capitalizes on the social aspects of learning. In Betty’s Brain, students instruct a character called a Teachable Agent (TA) which can reason based on how it is taught. Two studies demonstrate the protégé effect: students make greater effort to learn for their TAs than they do for themselves. The first study involved 8th-grade students learning biology. Although all students worked with the same Betty’s Brain software, students in the TA condition believed they were teaching their TAs, while in another condition, they believed they were learning for themselves. TA students spent more time on learning activities (e.g., reading) and also learned more. These beneficial effects were most pronounced for lower achieving children. The second study used a verbal protocol with 5th-grade students to determine the possible causes of the protégé effect. As before, students learned either for their TAs or for themselves. Like study 1, students in the TA condition spent more time on learning activities. These children treated their TAs socially by attributing mental states and responsibility to them. They were also more likely to acknowledge errors by displaying negative affect and making attributions for the causes of failures. Perhaps having a TA invokes a sense of responsibility that motivates learning, provides an environment in which knowledge can be improved through revision, and protects students’ egos from the psychological ramifications of failure.

Keywords

Educational technology Motivation K-12 education Peer tutoring 

References

  1. Ames R (1975) Teacher’s attributions of responsibility: some unexpected non-defensive effects. J Educ Psychol 67(5):668–676. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.67.5.668 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arena DA, Schwartz DL, Bailenson JN (2009) Effects of social belief on spatial learning in virtual reality. Paper presented to the Games, Learning and Society Conference 5.0, Madison, WIGoogle Scholar
  3. Bailenson JN, Blascovich J (2004) Avatars. In: Bainbridge WS (ed) Berkshire encyclopedia of human-computer interaction, vol 1. Berkshire Publishing Group, Great Barrington, MA, pp 64–68Google Scholar
  4. Baylor AL (2007) Pedagogical agents as a social interface. Educ Technol 47(1):11–14Google Scholar
  5. Baylor AL, Kim Y (2005) Simulating instructional roles through pedagogical agents. Int J Artif Intell Educ 15(2):95–115Google Scholar
  6. Biswas G, Leelawong K, Schwartz DL, Vye N, TAG-V (2005) Learning by teaching: a new agent paradigm for educational software. Appl Artif Intell 19:363–392. doi:10.1080/08839510590910200 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Blascovich J, Loomis J, Beall A, Swinth K, Hoyt C, Bailenson JN (2002) Immersive virtual environment technology as a methodological tool for social psychology. Psychol Inq 13:103–124. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1302_01 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chen J, Shohamy D, Ross V, Reeves B, Wagner AD (2009) The impact of social belief on the neurophysiology of learning and memory. Abstracts from the Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience. San Francisco, CAGoogle Scholar
  9. Chi MTH, Roy M, Hausmann RGM (2008) Observing tutorial dialogues collaboratively: insights about human tutoring effectiveness from vicarious learning. Cogn Sci 32:301–348. doi:10.1080/03640210701863396 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Clarebout G, Elen J, Johnson WL, Shaw E (2002) Animated pedagogical agents: an opportunity to be grasped? J Educ Multimed Hypermedia 11(3):267–286Google Scholar
  11. Davachi L, Mitchell JP, Wagner AD (2003) Multiple routes to memory: distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100(4):2157–2162. doi:10.1073/pnas.0337195100 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dweck CS (2000) Self-theories: their role in motivation, personality, and development. Taylor & Francis, PhiladelphiaGoogle Scholar
  13. Elliot E, Dweck C (1988) Goals: an approach to motivation and achievement. J Pers Soc Psychol 54(1):5–12. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gerhard M, Moore D, Hobbs D (2004) Embodiment and copresence in collaborative interfaces. Int J Hum Comput Stud 61(4):453–480. doi:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2003.12.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Imbert R, de Antonio A (2000) The Bunny Dilemma: stepping between agents and avatars. In: Proceedings of the 17th Twente workshop on language and technologyGoogle Scholar
  16. Kuhl PK, Tsao F-M, Liu H-M (2003) Foreign-language experience in infancy: effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci 100:9096–9101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Lester JC, Converse SA, Kahler SE, Barlow ST, Stone BA, Bhogal RS (1997) The persona effect: affective impact of animated pedagogical agents. In: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, pp. 359–366Google Scholar
  18. Moreno R, Mayer RE, Spires HA, Lester JC (2001) The case for social agency in computer-based teaching: do students learn more deeply when they interact with animated pedagogical agents? Cogn Instr 19:177–213. doi:10.1207/S1532690XCI1902_02 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Okita SY, Bailenson J, Schwartz DL (2007) The mere belief of social interaction improves learning. In: McNamara DS, Trafton JG (eds) The proceedings of the 29th meeting of the cognitive science society. August, Nashville, pp 1355–1360Google Scholar
  20. Perkins K, Adams W, Dubson M, Finkelstein N, Reid S, Wieman C, LeMaster R (2006) PhET: interactive simulations for teaching and learning physics. Phys Teach 44(1):18–23. doi:10.1119/1.2150754 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Pesce M (2000) The playful world: interactive toys and the future of imagination. Ballantine Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  22. Reeves B, Nass C (1998) The media equation: how people treat computers, television, and new media like real people and places. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  23. Renkl A (1995) Learning for later teaching: an exploration of mediational links between teaching expectancy and learning results. Learn Instr 5:21–36. doi:10.1016/0959-4752(94)00015-H CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Ross L, Bierbrauer G, Polly S (1974) Attribution of educational outcomes by professional and nonprofessional instructors. J Pers Soc Psychol 29(5):609–618. doi:10.1037/h0036740 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Ryokai K, Vaucelle C, Cassell J (2003) Virtual peers as partners in storytelling and literacy learning. J Comput Assist Learn 19:195–208. doi:10.1046/j.0266-4909.2003.00020.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Schwartz DL, Blair KP, Biswas G, Leelawong K, Davis J (2007) Animations of thought: interactivity in the teachable agents paradigm. In: Lowe R, Schnotz W (eds) Learning with animation: research and implications for design. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 114–140Google Scholar
  27. Schwartz DL, Chase C, Chin DB, Oppezzo M, Kwong H, Okita S, Biswas G, Roscoe RD, Jeong H, Wagster JD (2009) Interactive metacognition: monitoring and regulating a teachable agent. In: Handbook of metacognition in education (in press)Google Scholar
  28. Shimoda TA, White BY, Frederiksen JR (2002) Student goal orientation in learning inquiry skills with modifiable software advisors. Sci Educ 86:244–263. doi:10.1002/sce.10003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Turing AM (1950) Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind 59(236):433–460. doi:10.1093/mind/LIX.236.433 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Turkle S (1995) Life on the screen: identity in the age of the internet. Simon and Schuster, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  31. Wagster J, Tan J, Wu Y, Biswas G, Schwartz DL (2007) Do learning by teaching environments with metacognitive support help students develop better learning behaviors? In: The Proceedings of the 29th Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 695–700). August, Nashville, USAGoogle Scholar
  32. Weizenbaum J (1976) Computer power and human reason. W. H. Freeman, San Francisco, CAGoogle Scholar
  33. Yee N, Bailenson J (2007) The proteus effect: the effect of transformed self-representation on behavior. Hum Commun Res 33:271–290. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2007.00299.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Catherine C. Chase
    • 1
  • Doris B. Chin
    • 1
  • Marily A. Oppezzo
    • 1
  • Daniel L. Schwartz
    • 1
  1. 1.School of EducationStanford UniversityStanfordUSA

Personalised recommendations