Advertisement

Journal of Religion and Health

, Volume 58, Issue 3, pp 737–747 | Cite as

Enhancing Human Cognition Through Vajrayana Practices

  • Maria KozhevnikovEmail author
Original Paper

Abstract

Phenomenological studies suggest the existence of enhanced cognitive states, termed flow or peak experiences, in which specific cognitive processes (e.g., attention, perception) can be dramatically increased for limited durations. Here we review new scientific evidence that shows that specific types of meditation that developed out of certain religious traditions such as Vajrayana (Tantric Buddhism) and Hindu Tantra lead to the enhanced cognitive states, characterized by heightened sympathetic activation and phasic alertness (a significant temporary boost in focused attention). This is in contrast to the meditation practices (Shamatha, Vipassana) from other traditions such as Theravada and Mahayana that elicit heightened parasympathetic activity and tonic alertness. Such findings validate Buddhist scriptural descriptions of heightened arousal during Vajrayana practices and a calm and alert state of mind during Theravada and Mahayana types of meditation. The finding demonstrates the existence of enhanced cognitive states—the unique and energized states of consciousness characterized by a dramatic boost in focused attention.

Keywords

Enhanced cognitive state Vajrayana Buddhism Meditative practices Creativity Autonomic nervous system Arousal 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The author declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were approved by the IRB committee of the National University of Singapore, NUS-IRB Ref Code: N-17-007.

References

  1. Agee, J. D., Danoff-Burg, S., & Grant, C. A. (2009). Comparing brief stress management courses in a community sample: Mindfulness skills and progressive muscle relaxation. Explore, 104–109, 2009.Google Scholar
  2. Amihai, I., & Kozhevnikov, M. (2014). Arousal vs. relaxation: A comparison of the neurophysiological and cognitive correlates of Vajrayana and Theravada meditative practices. PLoS ONE, 9(7), e102990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Amihai, I., & Kozhevnikov, M. (2015). The influence of Buddhist meditation traditions on the autonomic system and attention. BioMed Research International, 2015, Article ID 731579.  https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/731579.
  4. Benson, H., Lehmann, J. W., Malhotra, M. S., Goldman, R. F., Hopkins, J., & Epstein, M. D. (1982). Body temperature changes during the practice of g Tum-mo yoga. Nature, 295, 234–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Benson, H., Malhotra, M. S., Goldman, R. F., Jacobs, G. D., & Hopkins, P. J. (1990). Three case reports of the metabolic and electroencephalographic changes during advanced Buddhist meditation techniques. Behavioral Medicine, 16, 90–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Benson, H., & Proctor, W. (2011). Relaxation revolution: The science and genetics of mind body healing. New York: Scribner.Google Scholar
  7. Benson, H., Rosner, B. A., Marzetta, B. R., & Klemchuk, H. P. (1974). Decreased blood pressure in borderline hypertensive subjects who practiced meditation. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 27, 163–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Berntson, G. G., Bigger, J. T., Jr., Eckberg, D. L., et al. (1997). Heart rate variability: Origins methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology, 34, 623–648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bhikkhu, B. (2012). The numerical discourses of the Buddha: A translation of the Anguttara Nikaya. Boston: Wisdom Publications.Google Scholar
  10. Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 11483–11488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Britton, W. B., Lindahl, J. R., Cahn, B. R., Davis, J. H., & Goldman, R. E. (2014). Awakening is not a metaphor: The effects of Buddhist meditation practices on basic wakefulness. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1307(1), 64–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Buksbazen, D. (2002). Zen Meditation in Plain English (1st ed.). Boston: Wisdom Publications.Google Scholar
  13. Camm, A. J., Malik, M., Bigger, J. T., Breithardt, G., Cerutti, S., Cohen, R. J., et al. (1996). Heart rate variability—standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation, 93, 1043–1065.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2009). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for stress management in healthy people: A review and meta-analysis. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 15, 593–600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2010). A systematic review of neurobiological and clinical features of mindfulness meditations. Psychological Medicine, 40, 1239–1252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Corby, J. C., Roth, W. T., Zarcone, V. P., Jr., & Kopell, B. S. (1978). Psychophysiological correlates of the practice of Tantric Yoga meditation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 571–577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Play and intrinsic rewards. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 15(3), 41–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow : The psychology of optimal experience (1st ed.). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  19. Gethin, R. (2011). On some definitions of mindfulness. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 263–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Getzels, J. W., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The creative vision: A longitudinal study of problem finding in art. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  21. Gilpin, R. (2008). The use of Theravāda Buddhist practices and perspectives in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Contemporary Buddhism, 9, 227–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 57, 35–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grossman, P., & Van Dam, N. T. (2011). Mindfulness, by any other name…: Trials and tribulations of sati in western psychology and science. Contemporary Buddhism: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 219–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Ho, C., Mason, O., & Spence, C. (2007). An investigation into the temporal dimension of the Mozart effect: Evidence from the attentional blink task. Acta Psychologica, 125, 117–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Jain, S., Shapiro, S. L., Swanick, S., et al. (2007). A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation versus relaxation training: effects on distress, positive states of mind, rumination, and distraction. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33, 11–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Keller, J., & Bless, H. (2008). Flow and regulatory compatibility: An experimental approach to the flow model of intrinsic motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 196–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kozhevnikov, M., Elliott, J., Shephard, J., & Gramann, K. (2013). Neurocognitive and somatic components of temperature increases during g-Tummo meditation: Legend and reality. PLoS ONE, 8(3), e58244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kozhevnikov, M., Li, Y., Wong, S., Obana, T., & Amihai, I. (2018). Do enhanced states exist? Boosting cognitive capacities through an action video-game. Cognition, 173, 93–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kozhevnikov, M., Louchakova, O., Josipovic, Z., & Motes, M. A. (2009). The enhancement of visuospatial processing efficiency through Buddhist deity meditation. Psychological Science, 20, 645–653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kutz, I., Leserman, J., Dorrington, C., Morrison, C. H., Borysenko, J. Z., & Benson, H. (1985). Meditation as an adjunct to psychotherapy. An outcome study. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 43, 209–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Low, P. (2013). Autonomic nervous system. In R. S. Porter & J. L. Kaplan (Eds.), The Merck manual. Whitehouse Station, NJ: Merck Sharp & Dohme.Google Scholar
  32. Lutz, A. (2007). Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness: An introduction. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 499–551). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. MacLean, K. A., Ferrer, E., Aichele, S. R., et al. (2010). Intensive meditation training improves perceptual discrimination and sustained attention. Psychological Science, 21, 829–839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Maslow, A. H. (1999). Toward a psychology of being (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  35. May, C. J., Burgard, M., Mena, M., Abbasi, I., Bernhardt, N., Clemens, S., et al. (2011). Short-term training in loving-kindness meditation produces a state, but not a trait, alteration of attention. Mindfulness, 2, 143–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Minvaleev, R. S., Bogdanov, A. R., Bogdanov, R. R., Bahner, D. P., & Marik, P. E. (2014). Hemodynamic observations of Tumo Yoga practitioners in a Himalayan Environment. The Journal of Alternative and Complimentary Medicine, 20, 295–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Moller, A. C., Meier, B. P., & Wall, R. D. (2010). Developing an experimental induction of flow: Effortless action in the lab. In B. Bruya (Ed.), Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action (pp. 191–204). London: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Morrison, S. F., & Blessing, W. W. (2011). Central nervous system regulation of body temperature. In I. J. Llewellyn-Smith & A. J. M. Verberne (Eds.), Central regulation of autonomic functions (pp. 1–34). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Morse, D. R., Martin, J. S., Furst, M. L., & Dubin, L. L. (1997). A physiological and subjective evaluation of meditation, hypnosis, and relaxation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 39, 304–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Nedergaard, J., Bengtsson, T., & Cannon, B. (2007). Unexpected evidence for active brown adipose tissue in adult humans. The American Journal of Physiology—Endocrinology and Metabolism, 293, E444–E452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after”. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, K. N. (1993). Music and spatial task performance. Nature, 365(6447), 611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Sturm, W., de Simone, A., Krause, B. J., et al. (1999). Functional anatomy of intrinsic alertness: Evidence for a fronto-parietal-thalamic-brainstem network in the right hemisphere”. Neuropsychologia, 37, 797–805.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sturm, W., & Willmes, K. (2001). On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. NeuroImage, S14, S76–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Telles, S., & Desiraju, T. (1993). Autonomic changes in Brahmakumaris Raja yoga meditation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, 147–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Tipitaka: The Pali Canon. (2005). Legacy Edition. http://www.accesstoinsight.org/tipitaka/.
  47. Tulku Urgyen Rinpoche. (1999). As it is (Vol. 2). Hong Kong: Ranjung Yeshe Publications.Google Scholar
  48. Wallace, R. K., & Benson, H. (1972). The physiology of meditation. Scientific American, 226, 84–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Weinbach, N., & Henik, A. (2011). Phasic alertness can modulate executive control by enhancing global processing of visual stimuli. Cognition, 121, 454–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Wilson, C. (1972). New pathways in psychology, Maslow and the post-Freudian revolution. New York: Taplinger Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  51. Zeidan, F., Martucci, K. T., Kraft, R., McHaffie, J. G., & Coghill, R. C. (2014). Neural correlates of mindfulness meditation-related anxiety relief. Scandinavica, 9, 751–759.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Psychology Department, Faculty of Arts and Social SciencesNational University of SingaporeSingaporeSingapore

Personalised recommendations