Advertisement

Journal of Religion and Health

, Volume 58, Issue 1, pp 271–288 | Cite as

A Data Mining and Data Visualization Approach to Examine the Interrelationships Between Life Satisfaction, Secularization and Religiosity

  • Chong Ho YuEmail author
  • Hailey Trier
  • Maryann Slama
Original Paper
  • 75 Downloads

Abstract

Previous studies have suggested a positive relationship between secularization and happiness, as well as a geographical, cultural and development pattern, primarily based on data gathered in Europe and the USA. To gain a more holistic view on the interrelationships between secularization, religiosity and subjective perception of well-being, this study utilized the Wave 6 archival data set (2010–2014) of World Values Survey, which contains 74,042 observations from 60 countries. In this study, the rating of satisfaction with life was treated as the dependent variable. Four secular value indices and 12 variables related to religiosity were extracted from the data set for predictive analysis. Data mining tools, such as the partition tree and bootstrap forest approaches, suggested that only secular values were influential. Specifically, secular values had a negative relationship with satisfaction. In addition, hierarchical clustering based on secularization and satisfaction did not suggest a meaningful pattern. For example, the dendrogram showed that South Korea, Lebanon, Estonia and Algeria were grouped together. This implies that secularization and satisfaction could vary from country to country, regardless of their geographical location, culture and development status. Specifically, countries that are similar in terms of satisfaction and secularization are not necessarily culturally similar or geographically clustered. This discourages the idea that these factors (e.g., location, development status, culture) play a role in mediating the relationship between secularization and religiosity. By directly contradicting previous work showing a pattern based on Europe and the USA, this finding challenges the existing understanding of the relationship between these factors. By expanding the scope of study to the whole world, the current analysis suggested that the existing view regarding the positive relationship between secularization and well-being might be oversimplified.

Keywords

Secularization Religiosity Well-being Data mining Big data analytics Happiness Mental health 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors did not receive any funding for this project, and thus, we declare that we have no conflict of interest with any party.

Ethical Approval

This article is based on archival data and does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors.

References

  1. Bénézech, M. (2011). Pathological happiness: On pleasant or joyous hallucinatory states and delusions. Annales Médico Psychologiques, 169, 209–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Berger, P. (2014). The many altars of modernity: Toward a paradigm for religion in a pluralist age. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berger, P., Davie, G., & Fokas, E. (2008). Religious America, secular Europe? A theme and variations. Burlington, VT: Ashgate.Google Scholar
  4. Bond, T. G., & Fox, C. M. (2015). Applying the Rasch model: Fundamental measurement in the human sciences. New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bonini, A. N. (2008). Cross-national variation in individual life satisfaction: Effects of national wealth, human development, and environmental conditions. Social Indicators Research, 87, 223–236.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-007-9167-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bowland, S., Edmond, T., & Fallot, R. D. (2012). Evaluation of a spiritually focused intervention with older trauma survivors. Social Work, 57(1), 73–82.  https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/swr001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Breiman, L. (2001). Random forests. Machine Learning, 45, 5–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Breiman, L. (2004). Consistency for a simple model of random forests (Technical report 670), UC Berkeley. https://www.stat.berkeley.edu/~breiman/RandomForests/consistencyRFA.pdf. Accessed 26 Sept 2018.
  9. Breiman, L., Friedman, J. H., Olshen, R. A., & Stone, C. J. (1984). Classification and regression trees. Monterey, CA: Wadsworth International Group.Google Scholar
  10. Bruce, S. (2002). God is dead: Secularization in the West. New York, NY: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  11. Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Coyne, J. (2015). Faith versus fact: Why science and religion are incompatible. New York, NY: Viking.Google Scholar
  13. de Tocqueville, A. (1840/2002). Democracy in America. Chicano, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  14. de Waal, F. (2014). The bonobo and the atheist. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
  15. Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Eberstadt, M. (2013). How the West really lost God: A new theory of secularization. West Conshohocken, PA: Templeton Press.Google Scholar
  17. Fadaia, B. (2013). Random forest on grouped data. Cross validation. https://stats.stackexchange.com/questions/19803/random-forest-on-grouped-data. Accessed 25 Sept 2018.
  18. Falk, C. F., & Cai, L. (2016). Maximum marginal likelihood estimation of a monotonic polynomial generalized partial credit model with applications to multiple group analysis. Psychometrika, 81, 434–460.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-014-9428-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Francis, L., & Stubbs, M. (1987). Measuring attitudes towards Christianity: From childhood into adulthood. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 741–743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Freedman, D. (1999). Ecological inference and the ecological fallacy (Technical report 549), UC Berkeley. http://statistics.berkeley.edu/sites/default/files/tech-reports/549.pdf. Accessed 27 Sept 2018.
  21. Galen, L. (2015). Atheism, wellbeing, and the wager: Why not believing in God (with others) is good for you. Science, Religion and Culture, 2(3), 54–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gallup, Inc. (2014). Gallup global wellbeing. https://news.gallup.com/poll/126965/gallup-global-wellbeing.aspx. Accessed 15 Dec 2016.
  23. Goldberg, D. (1992). General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12). Windsor: NFER-NELSON.Google Scholar
  24. Green, M., & Elliott, M. (2010). Religion, health, and psychological well-being. Journal of Religion and Health, 49, 149–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Grzegorzewski, P. (2012). Statistical tests for comparing Pareto charts. Communications in Numerical Analysis, 2012, 1–12.  https://doi.org/10.5899/2012/cna-00110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hayward, R. D., Krause, N., Ironson, G., Hill, P. C., & Emmons, R. (2016). Health and well-being among the non-religious: Atheists, agnostics, and no preference compared with religious group members. Journal of Religion and Health, 55(3), 1024–1037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hill, P. C., & Pargament, K. I. (2008). Advances in the conceptualization and measurement of religion and spirituality: Implications for physical and mental health research. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, S(1), 3–17.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1941-1022.S.1.3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Inglehart, R., & Welzel, C. (2010). Changing mass priorities: The link between modernization and democracy. Perspectives on Politics, 8, 551–567.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S1537592710001258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kistler, D., Thöni, C., & Welzel, C. (2017). Survey response and observed behavior: Emancipative and secular values predict prosocial behaviors. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 48, 461–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Klimberg, R., & McCullough, B. D. (2013). Fundamentals of predictive analytics with JMP. Cary, NC: SAS Institute.Google Scholar
  31. Koenig, H. G., Meador, K. G., & Parkerson, G. (1997). Religion index for psychiatric research. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 885–886.Google Scholar
  32. Li, L. M. W., & Bond, M. H. (2010). Does individual secularism promote life satisfaction? The moderating role of societal development. Social Indicators Research, 99, 443–453.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-010-9591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lincare, M. (2015). The sin of false precision: Too many rating-scale categories. Rasch Measurement Transactions, 28(2), 1463.Google Scholar
  34. Lynch, J. (2012). Religiosity: Investigating its affect on self-esteem, perceived stress, and general health in undergraduate students (Bachelor’s Thesis, Dublin Business School, Ireland). https://esource.dbs.ie/handle/10788/433. Accessed 15 Oct 2017.
  35. New Economic Foundation. (2015). Happy Planet Index. http://www.happyplanetindex.org/.
  36. Norris, P., & Inglehart, R. (2012). Sacred and secular: Religion and politics worldwide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Paul, G. (2014a). The health of nations. Skeptic, 19(3), 10–16.Google Scholar
  38. Paul, S. (2014b). The science of religion. http://www.gspaulscienceofreligion.com/. Accessed 15 Dec 2016.
  39. Pollack, D. (2008). Religious change in Europe: Theoretical considerations and empirical findings. Social Compass, 55(2), 168–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Rosenburg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Ryff, C. D. (2014). Psychological well-being revisited: Advances in science and practice. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 83, 10–28.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000353263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 719–727.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Sabatier, C., Mayer, B., Friedlmeier, M., Lubiewska, K., & Trommsdorff, G. (2011). Religiosity, family orientation, and life satisfaction of adolescents in four countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 1375–1393.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022111412343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. SAS Institute. (2016). JMP 13 [Computer Software]. Cary, NC: SAS Institute.Google Scholar
  45. Sinnewe, E., Kortt, M. A., & Dollery, B. (2015). Religion and life satisfaction: Evidence from Germany. Social Indicators Research, 123, 837–855.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-014-0763-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Speybroeck, N. (2012). Classification and regression trees. International Journal of Public Health, 57, 243–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Stark, R. (1999). Secularization, R.I.P. Society of Religion, 60, 249–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. United Nations Development Programme. (2015). Human Development Index. http://hdr.undp.org/en/content/human-development-index-hdi. Accessed 15 Dec 2016.
  49. Vanitha, A., & Niraimathi, S. (2013). Study on decision tree: Competent data classification. International Journal of Computer Science and Mobile Computing, 2, 365–370.Google Scholar
  50. Welzel, C. (2013). Freedom rising: Human empowerment and the quest for emancipation. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Welzel, C., & Delhey, J. (2015). Generalizing trust: The benign force of emancipation. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46, 875–896.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wilson, B. (1969). Religion in secular society. Harmondsworth: Pelican.Google Scholar
  53. Wilson, B. (1976a). Contemporary transformations of religion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Wilson, B. (1976b). Aspects of secularization in the West. Japanese Journal of Religious Studies, 3, 259–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Wilson, B. (1982). Religion in sociological perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  56. Wilson, B. (1994). If religion declines, will the social order survive? Onze Alma Mater, 48, 85–96.Google Scholar
  57. World Values Survey Association. (2014). World values survey. http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/wvs.jsp. Accessed 15 Dec 2016.
  58. Yu, C. H. (2010). Exploratory data analysis in the context of data mining and resampling. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(1), 9–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Yu, C. H., Reimer, D., Lee, A., Snijder, J., & Lee, H. S. (2017). A triangulated and exploratory study of the relationships between secularization, religiosity, and social wellbeing. Social Indicators Research, 131, 1103–1119.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-016-1290-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Zuckerman, P. (2008). Society without God. New York, NY: New York University Press.Google Scholar
  61. Zuckerman, P. (2012). Faith no more: Why people reject religion. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  62. Zuckerman, P. (2014). Living the secular life: New answers to old questions. New York, NY: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
  63. Zuckerman, P. (2015). Living the secular life: New answers to old questions. Lecture delivered at Skeptics Society, Pasadena, CA.Google Scholar
  64. Zullig, K. J., Ward, R. M., & Horn, T. (2006). The association between perceived spirituality, religiosity, and life satisfaction: The mediating role of self-rated health. Social Indicators Research, 79, 255–274.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-005-4127-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyAzusa Pacific UniversityAzusaUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyOxford UniversityOxfordEngland
  3. 3.Program of Industrial and Organizational PsychologySt. Mary’s UniversityHalifaxCanada

Personalised recommendations