Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 45, Issue 1, pp 121–141 | Cite as

Self-Guided Reading: Touch-Based Measures of Syntactic Processing

Article

Abstract

A novel online reading methodology termed Self-Guided Reading (SGR) is examined to determine if it can successfully detect well-studied syntactic processing behaviours. In SGR, a participant runs their finger under masked text in order to reveal a sentence. It is therefore similar to self-paced reading in presentation of stimuli, but different in the motion that the participant makes to interact with the stimuli. The phenomena of relative clause, adverb and noun phrase/sentential attachment are utilised to allow comparison to previous research that employed self-paced reading and eye-tracking. SGR was able to detect the predicted processing behaviours in all sentence types. Moreover, once design choices and task effects are accounted for, SGR was the most consistent in triggering a motor movement change at the predicted point in the sentence. Able to provide a semi-continuous reading measure at low cost, SGR should be investigated further to uncover the full potential of the method for psycholinguistic research.

Keywords

Reading Experimental methodology Relative clauses Touchscreen Syntactic processing 

References

  1. Altmann, G. T. M., van Nice, K. Y., Garnham, A., & Henstra, J.-A. (1998). Late closure in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 459–484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Author. (in press). The locus of processing for object relative clauses and the impact of methodology.Google Scholar
  3. Baayen, R. G., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baayen, R. H. (2011). languageR: Data sets and functions with analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics. R package version 1.2.Google Scholar
  5. Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2011). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-41.Google Scholar
  6. Brouwer, H., Fitz, H., & Hoeks, J. C. J. (2010). Modeling the noun phrase versus sentence coordination ambiguity in Dutch: Evidence from surprisal theory. In Proceedings of the 2010 ACL workshop on cognitive modeling and computational linguistics (ACL 10) (pp. 72–80), Uppsala, Sweden 15 July, 2010.Google Scholar
  7. Clifton, C., Staub, A., & Rayner, K. (2007). Eye movements in reading words and sentences. In R. V. Gompel, M. Fisher, W. Murray, & R. L. Hill (Eds.), Eye movements: A window on mind and brain (pp. 341–372). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Davis, C. J. (2010). The spatial coding model of visual word identification. psychological review, 117(3), 713.Google Scholar
  9. Dotan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2013). How do we convert a number into a finger trajectory? Cognition, 129, 512–529.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Dufau, et al. (2011). Smart phone, smart science: How the use of smartphones can revolutionize research in cognitive science. PLoS One, 6(9), 1–3.Google Scholar
  11. Finkbeiner, M., Coltheart, M., & Coltheart, V. (2014). Pointing the way to new constraints on the dynamical claims of computational models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(1), 172–185. doi:10.1037/a0033169.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Frazier, L. (1987). Syntactic processing: Evidence from Dutch. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 5, 519–559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Frazier, L., & Clifton, C, Jr. (1997). Construal: Overview, motivation, and some new evidence. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 277–295.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Freeman, J. B., Dale, R., & Farmer, T. A. (2011). Hand in motion reveals mind in motion. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gordon, P. C., & Lowder, M. W. (2012). Complex sentence processing: A review of theoretical perspectives on the comprehension of relative clauses. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6(7), 403–415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hoeks, J., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2002). Processing coordinated structures in context: The effect of topic-structure on ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 99–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Woolley, J. D. (1982). Paradigms and processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 228–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Levy, R. (2008). Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition, 106, 1126–1177.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Levy, R. (2011). Integrating surprisal and uncertain-input models in online sentence comprehension: Formal techniques and empirical results. In Proceedings of the 49th annual meeting of the association for computational linguistics.Google Scholar
  20. Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 314–324. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Mirman, D., Dixon, J. A., & Magnuson, J. A. (2008). Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 475–494.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Mitchell, D. C. (2004). On-line methods in language processing: Introduction and historical review. In M. Carreiras & C. Clifton (Eds.), The on-line study of sentence comprehension: Eye-tracking, ERP and beyond (pp. 15–32). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  23. Miyata, H., Itakura, S., & Fujita, K. (2009). Planning in human children (Homo sapiens) assessed by maze problems on the touch screen. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(1), 69–78.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Price, K. W., Meisinger, E. B., Louwerse, M. M., & D’Mello, S. K. (2012). Silent reading fluency using underlining: Evidence for an alternative method of assessment. Psychology in the Schools, 49(6), 606–618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Rayner, K., & Sereno, S. C. (1994). Eye movements in reading: Psycholinguistic studies. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (1st ed., pp. 57–81). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  26. Shvartsman, M., Lewis, R. L., & Singh, S. (2014). Computationally rational saccadic control: An explanation of spillover effects based on sampling from noisy perception and memory. In Proceedings of the 2014 ACL workshop on cognitive modeling and computational linguistics (pp. 1–9).Google Scholar
  27. Spivey, M. (2007). The continuity of mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Spivey, M. J., Grosjean, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). Continuous attraction towards phonological competitors. In Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the USA (vol. 102, pp. 10393–10398).Google Scholar
  29. Staub, A. (2011). Word recognition and syntactic attachment in reading: Evidence for a staged architecture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(3), 407–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268(5217), 1632–1634.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Kello, C. (1993). Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical preference from garden-paths. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19(3), 528–553.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Witzel, J., Witzel, N., & Nicol, J. (2012a). Deeper than shallow: Evidence for structure-based parsing biases in second-language sentence processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(2), 419–456.Google Scholar
  33. Witzel, N., Witzel, J., & Forster, K. I. (2012b). Comparisons of online reading paradigms: Eye tracking, moving-window, and maze. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 41, 105–128.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of English and LinguisticsUniversity of OtagoDunedinNew Zealand

Personalised recommendations