Advertisement

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 43, Issue 3, pp 225–240 | Cite as

Cross-Language Translation Priming Asymmetry with Chinese-English Bilinguals: A Test of the Sense Model

  • Baoguo ChenEmail author
  • Huixia Zhou
  • Yiwen Gao
  • Susan Dunlap
Article

Abstract

The present study aimed to test the Sense Model of cross-linguistic masked translation priming asymmetry, proposed by Finkbeiner et al. (J Mem Lang 51:1–22, 2004), by manipulating the number of senses that bilingual participants associated with words from both languages. Three lexical decision experiments were conducted with Chinese-English bilinguals. In Experiment 1, polysemous L2 words and their L1 Chinese single-sense translation equivalents were selected as primes and targets. In Experiment 2, single-sense L1 words and their L2 translation equivalents with polysemous senses severed as primes and targets. We found translation priming effects in the L1–L2 direction, but not in the L2–L1 direction. In Experiment 3, presentation time of the L2 priming stimulus was prolonged, and significant translation priming effects were observed in the L2–L1 direction. These findings suggest that the Sense Model does not adequately explain cross-language translation priming asymmetry. The sense numbers of primes and targets, as well as the activation proportion of these senses between them, were possibly not the primary reason for cross-language translation priming asymmetry. The revised hierarchical model (Kroll and Stewart in J Mem Lang 33:149–174, 1994) and the BIA+ model (Dijkstra and van Heuven in Bilingualism Lang Cognit 5:175–197, 2002) better explain the cross-language translation priming asymmetry we found.

Keywords

Cross-linguistic priming asymmetry Sense Model Second language 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the fund of The National Social Science Fund (11BYY039), the Central University Basic Scientific Research Fund, the Program for New Century Excellent Talents of Ministry of Education in China (NECT-11-0028) and the Beijing Key Lab of Applied Experimental Psychology.

References

  1. Basnight-Brown, D. M., & Altarriba, J. (2007). Differences in semantic and translation priming across languages: The role of language direction and language dominance. Memory & Cognition, 35, 953–965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977–990.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based on film subtitles. PloS One, 5(6), 10729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Davis, C., Sánchez-Casas, R., García-Albea, J., Guasch, M., Molero, M., & Ferré, P. (2010). Masked translation priming: Varying language experience and word type with Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 137–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. De Groot, A. M. B., & Nas, G. L. J. (1991). Lexical representation of cognates and noncognates in compound bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 90–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. de Groot, A. M. B. (1992a). Bilingual lexical representation: A closer look at conceptual representations. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 389–412). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. de Groot, A. M. B. (1992b). Determinants of word translation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, 1001–1018.Google Scholar
  8. Dijkstra, T., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5, 175–197.Google Scholar
  9. Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2011). Masked translation priming effects with low proficient bilinguals? Memory & Cognition, 39, 260–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Duñabeitia, J. A., Perea, M., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Masked translation priming effects with highly proficient simultaneous bilinguals. Experimental Psychology, 57(2), 98–107.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Duyck, W., & Brysbaert, M. (2004). Forward and backward number translation requires conceptual mediation in both balanced and unbalanced bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 30, 889–906.Google Scholar
  12. Duyck, W., & Warlop, N. (2009). Translation priming between the native language and a second language. Experimental Psychology, 56(3), 173–179.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Finkbeiner, M., Forster, K., Nicol, J., & Nakamura, K. (2004). The role of polysemy in masked semantic and translation priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gollan, T. H., Forster, K. I., & Frost, R. (1997). Translation priming with different scripts: Masked priming with cognates and noncognates in Hebrew-English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1122–1139.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Hino, Y., & Lupker, S. J. (1996). Effects of polysemy in lexical decision and naming: An alternative to lexical access accounts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(6), 1331–1356.Google Scholar
  16. Jiang, N. (1999). Testing processing explanations for the asymmetry in masked cross-language priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 59–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Jiang, N., & Forster, K. I. (2001). Cross-language priming asymmetries in lexical decision and episodic recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 32–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Keatley, C. W., Spinks, J. A., & De Gelder, B. (1994). Asymmetrical cross-language priming effects. Memory & Cognition, 22, 70–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Modern Chinese Dictionary. (1994). Beijing, China: The Commercial Press.Google Scholar
  21. Pexman, P. M., Holyk, G. G., & Monfils, M. H. (2003). Number-of-features effects and semantic processing. Memory & Cognition, 31, 842–855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Pexman, P. M., Hargreaves, I. S., Siakaluk, P. D., Bodner, G. E., & Pope, J. (2008). There are many ways to be rich: Effects of three measures of semantic richness on visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(1), 161–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Reyna, V. F. (1987). Understanding verbs: Easy extension, hard comprehension. In A. E. Ellis (Ed.), Progress in the psychology of language (pp. 301–315). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  24. Rodd, J., Gaskell, G., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2002). Making senses of semantic ambiguity: Semantic competition in lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 245–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Sánchez-Casas, R. M., García-Albea, J. E., & Davis, C. W. (1992). Bilingual lexical processing: Exploring the cognate/noncognate distinction. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4, 293–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Schoonbaert, S., Duyck, W., Brysbaert, M., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2009). Semantic and translation priming from a first language to a second and back: Making sense of the findings. Memory & Cognition, 37(5), 569–586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Taylor, I., & Taylor, M. M. (1990). Psycholinguistics: Learning and using language. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  28. Tokowicz, N., Kroll, J. F., De Groot, A. M. B., & Van Hell, J. G. (2002). Number-of-translation norms for Dutch-English translation pairs: A new tool for examining language production. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, Computers, 34, 435–451.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. van Hell, J. G., & de Groot, A. M. B. (1998). Conceptual representation in bilingual memory: Effects of concreteness and cognate status in word association. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 1, 193–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Wang, X., & Forster, K. (2010). Masked translation priming with semantic categorization: Test the Sense Model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(3), 327–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Baoguo Chen
    • 1
    Email author
  • Huixia Zhou
    • 1
  • Yiwen Gao
    • 1
  • Susan Dunlap
    • 2
  1. 1.School of PsychologyBeijing Normal UniversityBeijingChina
  2. 2.Learning Research and Development CenterUniversity of PittsburghPittsburghUSA

Personalised recommendations