Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 43, Issue 1, pp 59–80 | Cite as

Tracking Reading: Dual Task Costs of Oral Reading for Young Versus Older Adults

  • Susan Kemper
  • Daniel Bontempo
  • RaLynn Schmalzried
  • Whitney McKedy
  • Bruno Tagliaferri
  • Doug Kieweg
Article

Abstract

A digital pursuit rotor was used to monitor oral reading costs by time-locking tracking performance to the auditory wave form produced as young and older adults were reading out short paragraphs. Multilevel modeling was used to determine how paragraph-level predictors of length, grammatical complexity, and readability and person-level predictors such as speaker age or working memory capacity predicted reading and tracking performance. In addition, sentence-by-sentence variation in tracking performance was examined during the production of individual sentences and during the pauses before upcoming sentences. The results suggest that dual tasking has a greater impact on older adults’ reading comprehension and tracking performance. At the level of individual sentences, young and older adults adopt different strategies to deal with grammatically complex and propositionally dense sentences.

Keywords

Aging Linguistic processing Dual task demands  Reading 

References

  1. Brown, C., Snodgrass, T., Covington, M. A., Herman, R., & Kemper, S. (2008). Measuring propositional idea density through part-of-speech tagging. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 540–545.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Connelly, S. L., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1991). Age and reading: The impact of distraction. Psychology and Aging, 6, 533–541.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Engelhardt, P. E., Corley, M., Nigg, J. T., & Ferreira, F. (2010). The role of inhibition in the production of disfluencies. Memory and Cognition, 38, 617–628.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Faust, M. E., Balota, D. A., Spieler, D. H., & Ferraro, F. R. (1999). Individual differences in information-processing rate and amount: Implications for group differences in response latency. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 777–799.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Ferreira, F. (1991). Effects of length and syntactic complexity on initiation times for prepared utterances. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 210–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Flesch, R. F. (1948). A new readability yardstick. Journal of Applied Psychology, 32, 221–233.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Graesser, A. C., McNamara, D. S., Louwerse, M. M., & Cai, Z. (2004). Coh-Metrix: Analysis of text on cohesion and language. Behavior Reseach Methods, Instruments and Computers, 36, 193–202.Google Scholar
  9. Grober, E., & Sliwinski, M. (1991). Development and validation of a model for estimating premorbid verbal intelligence in the elderly. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 933–949.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 20, pp. 193–226). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  11. Hoffman, L., & Rovine, M. J. (2007). Multilevel models for the experimental psychologist: foundations and illustrative examples. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 110–117.Google Scholar
  12. Hummert, M. L. (2009). Group identity, age stereotypes, and communication in a life span context. In H. Giles, S. Reid, & J. Harwood (Eds.), Dynamics of intergroup communication (pp. 41–53). New York: Peter Lang Publishers.Google Scholar
  13. Kemper, S. (2006). Language in adulthood. In E. Bialystok & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change (pp. 223–238). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kemper, S., Bontempo, D., McKedy, W., Schmalzried, R., Tagliaferri, B., & Kieweg, D. (2010). Tracking sentence planning and production. The Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Science, 66, 160–168.Google Scholar
  15. Kemper, S., Hoffman, L., Schmalzried, R., Herman, R., Kieweg, D. (2011). Tracking talking: Dual task costs of planning and producing speech for young versus older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 18, 257–279.Google Scholar
  16. Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W., & Laham, D. (1998). An introduction to latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25, 259–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Levin, H., & Buckler-Addis, A. (1979). The eye-voice span. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  18. Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  19. Myerson, J., Adams, D. R., Hale, S., & Jenkins, L. (2003). Analysis of group differences in processing speed: Brinley plots, Q-Q plots, and other conspiracies. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10, 224–237.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 17, 299–320.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Rabaglia, C. D., & Salthouse, T. A. (2012). Natural and constrained language production as a function of age and cognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 1505–1531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ratcliff, R., Spieler, D., & McKoon, G. (2000). Explicitly modeling the effects of aging on response time. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 1–25.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271–276.Google Scholar
  24. Roland, D., Dick, F., & Elman, J. (2007). Frequency of basic English grammatical structures: A corpus analysis. Journal of Memory & Language, 57, 348–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Rosenberg, S., & Abbeduto, L. (1987). Indicators of linguistic competence in the peer group conversational behavior of mildly retarded adults. Applied Psycholinguistics, 8, 19–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Schneider, B. A., Daneman, M., & Murphy, D. R. (2005). Speech comprehension difficulties in older adults: Cognitive slowing or age-related changes in hearing? Psychology and Aging, 20, 261–271.Google Scholar
  27. Schneider, B. A., & Pichora Fuller, M. K. (2000). Implications of perceptual deterioration for cognitive aging research. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 155–219). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  28. Selkirk, E. O. (1986). Derived domains in sentence phonology. Phonology Yearbook, 3, 371–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Shipley, W. C. (1940). A self-administered scale for measuring intellectual impairment and deterioration. Journal of Psychology, 9, 371–377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Stata Corp. (2009). Stata statistical software: Release 11. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.Google Scholar
  31. Stine, E. A. L., Wingfield, A., & Poon, L. W. (1986). How much and how fast: Rapid processing of spoken language in later adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 1, 303–311.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., & Soederberg Miller, L. M. (2008). Self-regulated reading in adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 23, 131–153.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Swets, B., Desmet, T., Hambrick, D.Z., Ferreira, F. (2007). The role of working memory in syntactic ambiguity resolution: A psychometric approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 64–81.Google Scholar
  35. Tagliaferri, B. (2005). Paradigm. www.perceptionresearchsystems.com.
  36. Thornton, R., & Light, L. (2006). Language comprehension and production in normal aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (6th ed., pp. 261–287). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Tun, P. A. (1998). Fast Noisy Speech: Age differences in processing rapid speech with background noise. Psychology and Aging, 13, 424–434.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Verhaeghen, P., Steitz, D. W., Sliwinski, M. J., & Cerella, J. (2003). Aging and dual-task performance: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 18, 443–460.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Wechsler, D. (1958). The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Susan Kemper
    • 1
  • Daniel Bontempo
    • 2
  • RaLynn Schmalzried
    • 1
  • Whitney McKedy
    • 3
  • Bruno Tagliaferri
    • 2
  • Doug Kieweg
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Gerontology CenterUniversity of KansasLawrenceUSA
  2. 2.Center for Biobehavioral Neurosciences in Communication DisordersUniversity of KansasLawrenceUSA
  3. 3.Gerontology CenterUniversity of KansasLawrenceUSA

Personalised recommendations