Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 42, Issue 3, pp 205–225 | Cite as

Adjective Semantics, World Knowledge and Visual Context: Comprehension of Size Terms by 2- to 7-Year-Old Dutch-Speaking Children

Open Access
Article

Abstract

The interpretation of size terms involves constructing contextually-relevant reference points by combining visual cues with knowledge of typical object sizes. This study aims to establish at what age children learn to integrate these two sources of information in the interpretation process and tests comprehension of the Dutch adjectives groot ‘big’ and klein ‘small’ by 2- to 7-year-old children. The results demonstrate that there is a gradual increase in the ability to inhibit visual cues and to use world knowledge for interpreting size terms. 2- and 3-year-old children only used the extremes of the perceptual range as reference points. From age four onwards children, like adults, used a cut-off point in the mid-zone of a series. From age five on, children were able to integrate world knowledge and perceptual context. Although 7-year-olds could make subtle distinctions between sizes of various object classes, their performance on incongruent items was not yet adult-like.

Keywords

Size terms Language acquisition Reference points Adjectives 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The research reported in this paper was supported by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO), grant 446-08-018. I would like to thank all children, parents and teachers who made this investigation possible. I am also grateful to Martine Coene for help in setting up the experiment and Naomi Kamoen for insightful comments and practical assistance.

Open Access

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited.

References

  1. Barner D., Snedeker J. (2008) Compositionality and statistics in adjective acquisition: 4-Year-olds interpret tall and short based on the size distributions of novel noun referents. Child Development 79: 594–608PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bartlett E. J. (1975) Sizing things up: The acquisition of the meaning of dimensional adjectives. Journal of Child Language 3: 205–219Google Scholar
  3. Berndt R. S., Caramazza A. (1978) The development of vague modifiers in the language of pre-school children. Journal of Child Language 5: 279–294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bialystok E., Senman L. (2004) Executive processes in appearance-reality tasks: The role of inhibition of attention and symbolic representation. Child Development 75: 562–579PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bierwisch M. (1967) Some semantic universals of German adjectivals. Foundations of Language 3: 1–36Google Scholar
  6. Bierwisch M. (1989) The semantics of gradation. In: Bierwisch M., Lang E. (eds) Dimensional adjectives. Grammatical structure and conceptual interpretation. Springer, Berlin, pp 71–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Blackwell A. A. (2005) Acquiring the English adjective lexicon: Relationships with input properties and adjectival semantic typology. Journal of Child Language 32: 535–562PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bol G. W. (1995) Implicational scaling in child language acquisition: The order of production of Dutch verb constructions. In: Verrips M., Wijnen F. (eds) Papers from the Dutch–German colloquium on language acquisition. Institute for General Linguistics, Amsterdam, pp 1–13Google Scholar
  9. Brewer W., Stone J. B. (1975) Acquisition of spatial antonym pairs. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 19: 299–307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bryant D. J. (1997) Representing space in language and perception. Mind and Language 12: 239–264CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Clark H. H. (1970) The primitive nature of children’s relational concepts. In: Hayes J. R. (ed) Cognition and the development of language. Wiley, New York, pp 269–277Google Scholar
  12. Clark H. H. (1973) Space, time, semantics, and the child. In: Moore T. E. (ed) Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. Academic Press, New York and London, pp 27–63Google Scholar
  13. Costa A., Hernández M., Costa-Faidella J., Sebastián-Gallés N. (2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition 113: 135–149PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Coventry K. R., Carmichael R., Garrod S. C. (1994) Spatial prepositions, object-specific function, and task requirements. Journal of Semantics 11: 289–309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Coventry K. R., Garrod S. C. (2004) Saying, seeing and acting: The psychological semantics of spatial prepositions. Psychology Press, HoveGoogle Scholar
  16. Coventry K. R., Prat-Sala M., Richards L. (2001) The interplay between geometry and function in the comprehension of over, under, above, and below. Journal of Memory and Language 44: 376–398CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Davidson M. C., Amso D., Anderson L. C., Diamond A. (2006) Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory. Inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia 44: 2037–2078PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. De Mulder H. L. N. M. (2011) Putting the pieces together: The development of theory of mind and (mental) language. LOT, UtrechtGoogle Scholar
  19. Dirven R., Taylor J. R. (1988) The conceptualisation of vertical space in English: The case of tall. In: Rudzka-Ostyn B. (eds) Topics in cognitive linguistics. John Benjamins, Amsterdam and Philadelphia, pp 379–402Google Scholar
  20. Ebeling K. S., Gelman S. A. (1988) Coordination of size standards by young children. Child Development 59: 888–896PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ebeling K. S., Gelman S. A. (1994) Children’s use of context in interpreting “big” and “little”. Child Development 65: 1178–1192PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ehri L. C. (1976) Comprehension and production of adjectives and seriation. Journal of Child Language 3: 369–384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Eilers R. E., Oller D. K., Ellington J. (1974) The acquisition of word-meaning for dimensional adjectives: The long and short of it. Journal of Child Language 1: 195–204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fischer K., Roberts R. (1989) The development of classification skills in the preschool years: Developmental level and errors. Harvard University, HarvardGoogle Scholar
  25. Frye D., Zelazo P. D., Palfai T. (1995) Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development 10: 483–527CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Garon N., Bryson S. E., Smith I. M. (2008) Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin 134: 31–60PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Garrod S. C., Sandford A. J. (1989) Discourse models as interfaces between language and the spatial world. Journal of Semantics 6: 147–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gelman S. A., Ebeling K. S. (1989) Children’s use of nonegocentric standards in judgments of functional size. Child Development 60: 920–932PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gentner D. (1978) On relational meaning: The acquisition of verb meaning. Child Development 49: 988–998CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hagoort P., Hald L., Bastiaansen M., Petersson K. M. (2004) Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science 304: 438–441PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Holyoak K. J. (1978) Comparative judgments with numerical reference points. Cognitive Psychology 10: 203–243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kamp H. (1975) Two theories about adjectives. In: Keenan E. L. (ed) Formal semantics for natural languages. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 123–155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kanemura H., Aihara M., Araki T., Nakazawa S. (2003) Development of the prefrontal lobe in infants and children: A three-dimensional magnetic resonance volumetric study. Brain and Development 25: 195–199PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kennedy C. (2007) Vagueness and grammar: The semantics of relative and absolute gradable adjectives. Linguistics and Philosophy 30: 1–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Krawczyk D. C., Morrison R. G., Viskontas I., Holyoak K. J., Chow T. W., Mendez M. F., Miller B. L., Knowlton B. J. (2008) Distraction during relational reasoning: The role of prefrontal cortex in interference control. Neuropsychologia 46: 2020–2032PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Landau B., Smith L. B., Jones S. (1996) Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism. Cognition 60: 143–171PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Landau B., Smith L. B., Jones S. (1998) Object shape, object function, and object name. Journal of Memory and Language 38: 1–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Lang E. (1989) The semantics of dimensional designation of spatial objects. In: Bierwisch M., Lang E. (eds) Dimensional adjectives. Grammatical structure and conceptual interpretation. Springer, Berlin, pp 263–417CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Lehrer A., Lehrer K. (1982) Antonymy. Linguistics and Philosophy 5: 483–501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Leisi E. (1975) Der Wortinhalt: Seine Struktur im Deutschen und Englischen. Quelle and Meyer, HeidelbergGoogle Scholar
  41. Lyons J. (1977) Semantics. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. MacWhinney B. (2000) The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJGoogle Scholar
  43. Nelson K. (1976) Some attributes of adjectives used by young children. Cognition 4: 1–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Paivio A. (1975) Perceptual comparisons through the mind’s eye. Memory and Cognition 3(6): 635–647CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Ravid, D., Tribushinina, E., Korecky-Kröll, K., Xanthos, A., Kilani-Schoch, M., Laaha, S., et al. (2010). The first year of adjectives: A cross-linguistic study of the emergence of a category. Poster presented at the Child Language Seminar, London, June 24–25, 2010.Google Scholar
  46. Rips L. J., Turnbull W. (1980) How big is big? Relative and absolute properties in memory. Cognition 8: 145–174PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sapir E. (1944) Grading: A study in semantics. Philosophy of Science 11: 93–116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Sassoon G. W. (2011) Adjectival versus nominal categorization processes: The rule versus similarity hypothesis. In: Bochner G., De Brabanter P., Kissine M., Rossi D. (eds) Cognitive and empirical pragmatics: Issues and perspectives. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 104–147Google Scholar
  49. Saylor M. M. (2000) Time-stability and adjective use by child and adult English speakers. First Language 20: 91–120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Smith L. B., Cooney N. J., McCord C. (1986) What is “high”? The development of reference points for “high” and “low”. Child Development 57: 583–602PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Smith L. B., Jones S. S., Landau B. (1996) Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism?. Cognition 60: 143–171PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Smith L. B., Rattermann M. J., Sera M. (1988) “Higher” and “lower”: comparative and categorical interpretations by children. Cognitive Development 3: 341–357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Syrett K., Kennedy C., Lidz J. (2010) Meaning and context in children’s understanding of gradable adjectives. Journal of Semantics 27: 1–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Šetić M., Dominjan D. (2007) The influence of spatial orientation on property verification. Language and Cognitive Processes 22: 297–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Tomikawa S. A., Dodd D. H. (1980) Early word meanings: Perceptually or functionally based?. Child Development 51: 1103–1109PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Tribushinina E. (2008a) Cognitive reference points: Semantics beyond the prototypes in adjectives of space and colour. LOT, UtrechtGoogle Scholar
  57. Tribushinina E. (2008b) Do small elephants exist? Prototypicality effects in the acquisition of dimensional adjectives. Toegepaste Taalwetenschap in Artikelen 79(1): 31–41Google Scholar
  58. Tribushinina E. (2010) The cognitive status of the norm. In: Lushnikova G. I., Prokhorova L. P., Omelichkina S. V., Ermolaeva E. N., Sokolova N. S., Mokrushina E. J. (eds) Concept and culture. Kemerovo University Press, Kemerovo, pp 494–500Google Scholar
  59. Tribushinina E. (2011) Once again on norms and comparison classes. Linguistics 49(3): 525–553CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Tribushinina, E. (in press). Spatial adjectives in Dutch child language: Towards a usage-based model of adjective acquisition. In C. Paradis, J. Hudson, & U. Magnusson (Eds.), Conceptual spaces and the construal of spatial meaning: Empirical evidence from human communication. Oxford: Oxford University Press (to appear).Google Scholar
  61. Vogel A. (2004) Swedish dimensional adjectives. Almqvist and Wiksell International, StockholmGoogle Scholar
  62. Walz J. A., Knowlton B. J., Holyoak K. J., Boone K. B., Mishkin F. S., de Menezes Santos M., Thomas C. R., Miller B. L. (1999) A system for relational reasoning in human prefrontal cortex. Psychological Science 10: 119–125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Weydt H., Schlieben-Lange B. (1998) The meaning of dimensional adjectives. Discovering the semantic process. Lexicology 4(2): 199–236Google Scholar
  64. Zelazo P. D. (2004) The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 12–17PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Zelazo P. D., Craik F. I. M., Booth L. (2004) Executive function across the life span. Acta Psychologica 115: 167–183PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Zelazo P. D., Frye D. (1998) Cognitive complexity and control: The development of exectuive function. Current Directions in Psychological Science 7: 121–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Zelazo P. D., Frye D., Rapus T. (1996) An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development 11: 37–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Zelazo P. D., Reznik J. S. (1991) Age-related asynchrony of knowledge and action. Child Development 62: 719–735CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Dutch Language and Culture, Utrecht Institute of LinguisticsUtrecht UniversityUtrechtThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations