Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 37, Issue 1, pp 1–20 | Cite as

Definitions of Idioms in Preadolescents, Adolescents, and Adults

Article

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to expand the current literature on word definitions by focusing on definitions of idioms provided by several age groups. Preadolescents, young adolescents, older adolescents, and adults wrote definitions for 10 frequently used idioms and also rated their familiarity with the idiomatic expressions. Participants’ definitions were scored based on the degree to which their definitions reflected use of critical elements (determined by a standard dictionary of idioms), use of examples or related/associated concepts, and errors. Significant age differences were found in both idiom familiarity and idiom definition tasks: both idiom familiarity and definitional skill improved with age. In addition, we found a positive correlation between idiom familiarity and idiom definition. Results are discussed with respect to age-related changes in definitional response types and understanding of figurative language.

Keywords

Idiom Definition Familiarity Figurative language 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ackerman B.P. (1982) On comprehending idioms: Do children get the picture?. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 33: 439–454CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Al-Issa I. (1969) The development of word definition in children. Journal of Genetic Psychology 114: 25–28Google Scholar
  3. Anglin J.M. (1977) Word, object, and conceptual development. Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  4. Brasseur J., Jimenez B.C. (1989) Performance of university students on the Fullerton subtest of idioms. Journal of Communication Disorders 22: 351–359PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cacciari C., Levorato M.C. (1989) How children understand idioms in discourse. Journal of Child Language 16: 387–405PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Carey S. (1978) The child as word learner. In: Halle M., Bresnan J., Miller G. (eds). Linguistic theory and psychological reality. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 264–293Google Scholar
  7. Chan, Y., & Marinellie, S. A. (2002). Comprehension strategies used by L2 learners in processing idioms. Paper presented at the annual convention of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
  8. Connine C.M., Mullennix J., Shernoff E., Yelen J. (1990) Word familiarity and frequency in visual and auditory word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16(6): 1084–1096PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Curtis M.E. (1987) Vocabulary testing and vocabulary instruction. In: McKeown M.G., Curtis M.E. (eds). The nature of vocabulary acquisition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 37–51Google Scholar
  10. Deák, G. O., & Wagner, J. H. (2003). “Slow mapping” in children’s learning of semantic relations. In R. Alterman & D. Kirsh (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 318–323), Boston, MA: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  11. Drum, P. A., & Konopak, B. C. (1987). Learning word meanings from written context. In M. G. McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 73–87). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  12. Durso F., Shore W. (1991) Partial knowledge of word meanings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 120(2): 190–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gardner H., Winner E. (1979) The child is father to the metaphor. Psychology Today 12: 81–91Google Scholar
  14. Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. Kuczaj II (Ed.), Language development, Volume 2: Language, thought and culture (pp. 301–334). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  15. Gernsbacher M.A. (1984) Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113(2): 256–281CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gibbs R.W. Jr. (1987) Linguistic factors in children’s understanding of idioms. Journal of Child Language 14: 569–586PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Gibbs R.W. Jr. (1991) Semantic analyzability in children’s understanding of idioms. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 34: 613–620PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Graesser A.C., Hopskinson P.L., Schmid C. (1987) Differences in interconcept organization between nouns and verbs. Journal of Memory and Language 26: 242–253CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Johnson C.J., Anglin J.M. (1995) Qualitative developments in the content and form of children’s definitions. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38: 612–629PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Katz J.J., Foder J.A. (1963) The structure of a semantic theory. Language 39(2): 170–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kerbel D., Grunwell P. (1997) Idioms in the classroom: An investigation of language unit and mainstream teachers’ use of idioms. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 13(2): 113–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Lazar R.T., Warr-Leeper G.A., Nicholson C.B., Johnson S. (1989) Elementary school teachers’ use of multiple meaning expressions. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 20: 420–430Google Scholar
  23. Levorato M.C., Cacciari C. (1992) Children’s comprehension and production of idioms: The role of context and familiarity. Journal of Child Language 19(2): 415–433PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Litowitz B. (1977) Learning to make definitions. Journal of Child Language 4: 289–304Google Scholar
  25. Makkai, A., Boatner, M. T., & Gates, J. E. (1995). A dictionary of American idioms (3rd ed.). NY: Barron’s Educational Series.Google Scholar
  26. Marinellie S.A., Chan Y. (2006) The effect of word frequency on noun and verb definitions: A developmental study. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 49(5): 1001–1021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Marinellie S.A., Johnson C.J. (2002) Definitional skills in school-age children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders 35: 241–259PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Marinellie S.A., Johnson C.J. (2003) Adjective definitions and the influence of word frequency. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 46(5): 1061–1076CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Marinellie S.A., Johnson C.J. (2004) Nouns and verbs: A comparison of definitional style. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 33(3): 217–235PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Markman E.M. (1989) Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  31. Markowitz J., Franz S.K. (1988) The development of defining style. International Journal of Lexicography 1(3): 253–267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. McGhee-Bidlack B. (1991) The development of noun definitions: A metalinguistic analysis. Journal of Child Language 18: 417–434PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. McGregor K.K., Friedman R.M., Reilly R.M., Newman R.M. (2002) Semantic representation and naming in young children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 45: 332–346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Miller G.A. (1996) The science of words. W. H. Freeman & Co., New YorkGoogle Scholar
  35. Nayak N.P., Gibbs R.W. Jr. (1990) Conceptual knowledge in the interpretation of idioms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 119(3): 315–330CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Nippold M.A. (1991) Evaluating and enhancing idiom comprehension in language-disordered Language. Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 22(3): 100–106Google Scholar
  37. Nippold, M. A. (1998). Later language development: The school-age and adolescent years (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
  38. Nippold M.A., Hegel S., Sohlberg K.M., Schwarz I.E. (1999) Defining abstract entities: Development in preadolescents, adolescents, and young adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42: 473–481PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Nippold M.A., Martin S.T. (1989) Idiom interpretation in isolation versus context: A developmental study with adolescents. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 32(1): 59–66PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Nippold M.A., Moran C., Schwarz I.E. (2001) Idiom understanding in preadolescents: Synergy in Action. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 10: 169–179CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Nippold M.A., Rudzinski M. (1993) Familiarity and transparency in idiom explanation: A developmental study of children and adolescents. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36(4): 728–737PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Nippold M.A., Taylor C.L. (1995) Idiom understanding in youth: Further examination of familiarity and transparency. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38(2): 426–433PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Nippold M.A., Taylor C.L., Baker J.M. (1996) Idiom understanding in Australian youth: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 39: 442–447PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Prinz P.M. (1983) The development of idiomatic meaning in children. Language and Speech 26(3): 263–271Google Scholar
  45. Qualls C.D., O’Brien R.M., Blood G.W., Hammer C.S. (2003a) Contextual variation, familiarity, academic literacy, and rural adolescents’ idiom knowledge. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 34: 69–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Qualls C.D., Treaster B., Blood G.W., Hammer C.S. (2003b) Lexicalization of idioms in urban fifth graders: A reaction time study. Journal of Communication Disorders 36: 245–261PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Rips L.J., Shoben E.J., Smith E.E. (1973) Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 12: 1–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Roberts R.M., Kreuz R.J. (1994) Why do people use figurative language?. Psychological Science 5(3): 159–163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Smith E.E., Medin D.L. (1981) Categories and concepts. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  50. Spector C.C. (1996) Children’s comprehension of idioms in the context of humor. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 27: 307–313Google Scholar
  51. Storck P.A., Looft W.R. (1973) Qualitative analysis of vocabulary responses from persons aged six to sixty-six plus. Journal of Educational Psychology 65: 192–197PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Swartz K., Hall A. (1972) Development of relational concepts and word definition in children five through eleven. Child Development 43(1): 239–244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Swinney D.A., Cutler A. (1979) The access and processing of idiomatic expressions. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18: 523–534CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Thorum A.R. (1980) The Fullerton language test for adolescents, experimental education. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CAGoogle Scholar
  55. U.S. Census Bureau. (2000). American FactFinder: Census 2000 demographic profile highlights. Retrieved June 23, 2005, from http://factfinder.census.gov/home/saff/main.html?_lang=en
  56. Watson R. (1985) Towards a theory of definition. Journal of Child Language 12: 181–197PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Wehren A., De Lisi R., Arnold M. (1981) The development of noun definition. Journal of Child Language 8: 165–175PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Werner H., Kaplan B. (1963) Symbol formation: An organismic-developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Hearing, Speech and Language SciencesOhio UniversityAthensUSA

Personalised recommendations