Advertisement

Mimicking Others’ Nonverbal Signals is Associated with Increased Attitude Contagion

  • Allison L. SkinnerEmail author
  • Adilene Osnaya
  • Bhumi Patel
  • Sylvia P. Perry
Original Paper

Abstract

Observing nonverbal signals being directed toward unfamiliar individuals is known to influence attitudes and behavior toward those individuals. Specifically, observing biased nonverbal signals in favor of one individual over another can produce nonverbal signal-consistent attitudes among preschool children. Research has also shown that people have a tendency to mimic the behavior of others. The phenomenon of mimicking another’s nonverbal emotional response and “catching” their emotions has long been established. However, it has yet to be examined whether this phenomenon is associated with attitude contagion. We hypothesized that preschool children who mimic the biased nonverbal signals of others will be more likely to adopt their social attitudes. Results of the current study indicated that as emotional mimicry became more frequent, children showed an increasingly greater probability of acquiring nonverbal signal-consistent attitudes. Moreover, the frequency of negative—but not positive—emotional mimicry was related to an increased probability of showing nonverbal signal-consistent attitudes. Our findings provide initial support for the notion that mimicking others’ biased nonverbal signals may help facilitate attitude contagion.

Keywords

Social bias Nonverbal signals Children Mimicry Emotional mimicry Attitude contagion 

Notes

Supplementary material

10919_2019_322_MOESM1_ESM.docx (13 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 13 kb)

References

  1. Bandura, A., & Rosenthal, T. L. (1966). Vicarious classical conditioning as a function of arousal level. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 54–62.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0022639.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Blairy, S., Herrera, P., & Hess, U. (1999). Mimicry and the judgment of emotional facial expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 5–41.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021370825283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bourgeois, P., & Hess, U. (2008). The impact of social context on mimicry. Biological Psychology, 77, 343–352.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.008.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Brey, E., & Shutts, K. (2018). Children use nonverbal cues from an adult to evaluate peers. Journal of Cognition and Development, 19, 121–136.  https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2018.1449749.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Buttelmann, D., Zmyj, N., Daum, M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants. Child Development, 84, 422–428.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01860.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Castelli, L., Carraro, L., Pavan, G., Murelli, E., & Carraro, A. (2012). The power of the unsaid: The influence of nonverbal cues on implicit attitudes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42, 1376–1393.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00903.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Castelli, L., De Dea, C., & Nesdale, D. (2008). Learning social attitudes: Children’s sensitivity to the nonverbal behaviors of adult models during interracial interactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1504–1513.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208322769.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.76.6.893.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Coles, N. A., Larsen, J. T., & Lench, H. (2017). A meta-analysis of the facial feedback hypothesis literature.  https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/svjru.
  11. Curran, P. J., & Hussong, A. M. (2009). Integrative data analysis: The simultaneous analysis of multiple datasets. Psychological Methods, 14, 81–100.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015914.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Davis, J. I., Senghas, A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2009). How does facial feedback modulate emotional experience? Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 822–829.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.06.005.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. de Rosnay, M., Cooper, P. J., Tsigaras, N., & Murray, L. (2006). Transmission of social anxiety from mother to infant: An experimental study using a social referencing paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1165–1175.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.09.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dimberg, U., & Lundquist, L. O. (1990). Gender differences in facial reactions to facial expressions. Biological Psychology, 30, 151–159.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(90)90024-Q.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Dovidio, J. F. (2009). Psychology. Racial bias, unspoken but heard. Science, 326(5960), 1641–1642.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1184231.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Duffy, K. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2015). Mimicry: Causes and consequences. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 112–116.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.03.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Automatic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221, 1208–1210.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.6612338.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Fawcett, C., & Liszkowski, U. (2012). Mimicry and play initiation in 18-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 689–696.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.07.014.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Geangu, E., Quadrelli, E., Conte, S., Croci, E., & Turati, C. (2016). Three-year-olds’ rapid facial electromyographic responses to emotional facial expressions and body postures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 144, 1–14.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.001.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Grossard, C., Chaby, L., Hun, S., Pellerin, H., Bourgeois, J., Dapogny, A., et al. (2018). Children facial expression production: Influence of age, gender, emotion subtype, elicitation condition and culture. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 446.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00446.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2010). 3-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Developmental Science, 13, 923–929.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00951.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1992). Primitive emotional contagion. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 151–177.Google Scholar
  23. Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Hatfield, E. C., Bensman, L., Thornton, P. D., & Rapson, R. L. (2014). New perspectives on emotional contagion: A review of classic and recent research on facial mimicry and contagion. Interpersona: An International Journal on Personal Relationships, 8, 159–179.  https://doi.org/10.5964/ijpr.v8i2.162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hawk, S. T., Fischer, A. H., & van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Face the noise: Embodied responses to nonverbal vocalizations of discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 796–814.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026234.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hess, U., & Bourgeois, P. (2010). You smile–I smile: Emotion expression in social interaction. Biological Psychology, 84, 514–520.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.11.001.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 142–157.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868312472607.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2014). Emotional mimicry: Why and when we mimic emotions. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8, 45–57.  https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2017). The role of emotional mimicry in intergroup relations. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication.  https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.013.433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Ipser, A., & Cook, R. (2015). Inducing a concurrent motor load reduces categorization precision for facial expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 706–718.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000177.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Isomura, T., & Nakano, T. (2016). Automatic facial mimicry in response to dynamic emotional stimuli in five-month-old infants. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 283(1844), 20161948.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2016.1948.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 887–900.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.887.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Kavanagh, L. C., & Winkielman, P. (2016). The functionality of spontaneous mimicry and its influences on affiliation: An implicit socialization account. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 458.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00458.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Kulesza, W. M., Cisłak, A., Vallacher, R. R., Nowak, A., Czekiel, M., & Bedynska, S. (2015). The face of the chameleon: The experience of facial mimicry for the mimicker and the mimickee. The Journal of Social Psychology, 155, 590–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Laird, J. D. (1984). The real role of facial response in the experience of emotion: A reply to Tourangeau and Ellsworth, and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 909–917.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.4.909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, 334–339.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025389814290.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Lakin, J. L., Chartrand, T. L., & Arkin, R. M. (2008). I am too just like you: Nonconscious mimicry as an automatic behavioral response to social exclusion. Psychological Science, 19, 816–822.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02162.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon effect as a social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 145–162.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025389814290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Lawrence, K., Campbell, R., & Skuse, D. (2015). Age, gender, and puberty influence the development of facial emotion recognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–14.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lehane, C. M. (2015). Male and female differences in nonconscious mimicry: A systematic review. Journal of European Psychology Students, 6(3), 34–48.  https://doi.org/10.5334/jeps/de.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Maringer, M., Krumhuber, E. G., Fischer, A. H., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2011). Beyond smile dynamics: Mimicry and beliefs in judgments of smiles. Emotion, 11, 181–187.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022596.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. McShane, B. B., & Böckenholt, U. (2017). Single paper meta-analysis: Benefits for study summary, theory-testing, and replicability. Journal of Consumer Research, 43, 1048–1063.  https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucw085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.198.4312.75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 83–99.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(94)90024-8.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Moody, E. J., Reed, C. L., Van Bommel, T., App, B., & McIntosh, D. N. (2018). Emotional mimicry beyond the face? Rapid face and body responses to facial expressions. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9, 844–852.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617726832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Murata, A., Saito, H., Schug, J., Ogawa, K., & Kameda, T. (2016). Spontaneous facial mimicry is enhanced by the goal of inferring emotional states: Evidence for moderation of “automatic” mimicry. Higher Cognitive Processes, 11(4), 1–15.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0153128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Neal, D. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2011). Embodied emotion perception: Amplifying and dampening facial feedback modulates emotion perception accuracy. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 673–678.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611406138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Nielsen, M., & Blank, C. (2011). Imitation in young children: When who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1050–1053.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023866.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Oberman, L. M., Winkielman, P., & Ramachandran, W. S. (2007). Face to face: Blocking facial mimicry can selectively impair recognition of emotional expressions. Social Neuroscience, 2(3–4), 167–178.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910701391943.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 296–320.  https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rychlowska, M., Cañadas, E., Wood, A., Krumhuber, E. G., Fischer, A., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2014). Blocking mimicry makes true and false smiles look the same. PLoS ONE, 9(3), E90876.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090876.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  53. Rymarczyk, K., Żurawski, Ł., Jankowiak-Siuda, K., & Szatkowska, I. (2016a). Do dynamic compared to static facial expressions of happiness and anger reveal enhanced facial mimicry? PLoS ONE, 11(7), e0158534.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158534.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  54. Rymarczyk, K., Żurawski, Ł., Jankowiak-Siuda, K., & Szatkowska, I. (2016b). Emotional empathy and facial mimicry for static and dynamic facial expressions of fear and disgust. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1853.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01853.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  55. Skinner, A. L., Meltzoff, A. N., & Olson, K. R. (2017). “Catching” social bias: Exposure to biased nonverbal signals creates social biases in preschool children. Psychological Science, 28, 216–224.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616678930.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Skinner, A. L., Olson, K. R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2019). Acquiring group bias: Observing other people’s nonverbal signals can create social group biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.  https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000218.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Sonnby-Borgstrom, M., Jonsson, P., & Svensson, O. (2003). Emotional empathy as related to mimicry reactions at different levels of information processing. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 3–23.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023608506243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Stel, M., & Vonk, R. (2010). Mimicry in social interaction: Benefits for mimickers, mimickees, and their interaction. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 311–323.  https://doi.org/10.1348/000712609X465424.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Tschacher, W., Rees, G. M., & Ramseyer, F. (2014). Nonverbal synchrony and affect in dyadic interactions. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Vaish, A., Grossmann, T., & Woodward, A. (2008). Not all emotions are created equal: The negativity bias in social-emotional development. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 383–403.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.383.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  61. van Schaik, J. E., & Hunnius, S. (2016). Little chameleons: The development of social mimicry during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 147, 71–81.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Weisbuch, M., Pauker, K., & Ambady, N. (2009). The subtle transmission of race bias via televised nonverbal behavior. Science, 326, 1711–1714.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1178358.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  63. Widen, S. C. (2013). Children’s interpretation of facial expressions: The long path from valence-based to specific discrete categories. Emotion Review, 5, 72–77.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912451492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Willard, G., Issac, K., & Carney, D. R. (2015). Some evidence for the nonverbal contagion of racial bias. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 128, 96–107.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2015.04.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Yabar, Y., Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Peace, V. (2006). Implicit behavioral mimicry: Investigating the impact of group membership. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30, 97–113.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-006-0010-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Allison L. Skinner
    • 1
    Email author
  • Adilene Osnaya
    • 2
  • Bhumi Patel
    • 1
  • Sylvia P. Perry
    • 3
  1. 1.Psychology DepartmentUniversity of GeorgiaAthensUSA
  2. 2.Department of Psychological SciencesPurdue UniversityWest LafayetteUSA
  3. 3.Psychology DepartmentNorthwestern UniversityEvanstonUSA

Personalised recommendations