Advertisement

Nonverbal Behavior of Persuasive Sources: A Multiple Process Analysis

  • Joshua J. GuyerEmail author
  • Pablo Briñol
  • Richard E. Petty
  • Javier Horcajo
Review Paper
  • 29 Downloads

Abstract

This article describes the basic mechanisms by which the nonverbal behavior of a communicator can influence recipients’ attitudes and persuasion. We review the literature on classic variables related to persuasive sources (e.g., physical attractiveness, credibility, and power), as well as research on mimicry and facial expressions of emotion, and beyond. Using the elaboration likelihood model (ELM) as a framework, we argue that the overt behavior of source variables can affect attitude change by different psychological processes depending on different circumstances. Specifically, we describe the primary and secondary cognitive processes by which nonverbal behaviors of the source (e.g., smiling, nodding, eye contact, and body orientation) affect attitude change. Furthermore, we illustrate how considering the processes outlined by the ELM can help to predict when and why attractive, credible, and powerful communicators can not only increase persuasion but also be detrimental for persuasion.

Keywords

Nonverbal behavior Body Source Attitudes Persuasion 

Notes

References

  1. Aguinis, H., & Henle, C. A. (2001). Effects of nonverbal behavior on perceptions of a female employee’s power bases. The Journal of Social Psychology, 141, 537–549.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Albert, S., & Dabbs, J. M. (1970). Physical distance and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15(3), 265–270.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Anderson, N. H. (1971). Integration theory and attitude change. Psychological Review, 78, 171–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Andrews, J. C., & Shimp, T. A. (1990). Effects of involvement, argument strength, and source characteristics on central and peripheral processing of advertising. Psychology & Marketing, 7(3), 195–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Argyle, M. (1988). Bodily communication (2nd ed.). Madison: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  7. Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 70(9), 1–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Axsom, D., Yates, S., & Chaiken, S. (1987). Audience response as a heuristic cue in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 30–40.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Bailenson, J., & Yee, N. (2005). Digital chameleons: Automatic assimilation of nonverbal gestures in immersive virtual environments. Psychological Science, 16, 814–819.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bayliss, A. P., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Affective evaluations of objects are influenced by observed gaze direction and emotion expression. Cognition, 104, 644–653.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  12. Bernstein, M. J., Young, S. G., Brown, C. M., Sacco, D. F., & Claypool, H. M. (2016). Adaptive responses to social exclusion: Social rejection improves detection of real and fake smiles. Psychological Science, 19(10), 981–983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Berry, D. S., & McArthur, L. Z. (1986). Perceiving character in faces: The impact of age-related craniofacial changes on social perception. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 3–18.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Boothby, E. J., Clark, M. S., & Bargh, J. A. (2014). Shared experiences are amplified. Psychological Science, 25(12), 2209–2216.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Briñol, P., & DeMarree, K. G. (Eds.). (2012). Social metacognition. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  16. Briñol, P., DeMarree, K. G., & Smith, K. R. (2010). The role of embodied change in perceiving and processing facial expressions of others. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 437–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2008). Embodied persuasion: Fundamental processes by which bodily responses can impact attitudes. In G. R. Semin & E. R. Smith (Eds.), Embodiment grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 184–207). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2009). Source factors in persuasion: A self-validation approach. European Review of Social Psychology, 20, 49–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2012). The history of attitudes and persuasion research. In A. Kruglanski & W. Stroebe (Eds.), Handbook of the history of social psychology (pp. 285–320). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  20. Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2018). The impact of individual differences on attitudes and attitude change. In D. Albarracín & B. T. Johnson (Eds.), Handbook of attitudes (pp. 520–556). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  21. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & Barden, J. (2007). Happiness versus sadness as determinants of thought confidence in persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 711–727.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & Belding, J. (2017a). Objectification of people and thoughts: An attitude change perspective. British Journal of Social Psychology, 56, 233–249.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & DeMarree, K. G. (2015). Being threatened and being a threat can increase reliance on thoughts: A self-validation approach. In P. J. Carroll, R. M. Arkin, & A. Wichman (Eds.), Handbook of personal security (pp. 37–54). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  24. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Durso, R. O., & Rucker, D. D. (2017b). Power and persuasion: Processes by which perceived power can influence evaluative judgments. Review of General Psychology, 21, 223–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & McCaslin, M. J. (2009). Changing attitudes on implicit versus explicit measures: What is the difference? In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures (pp. 285–326). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  26. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Santos, D., & Mello, J. (2018). Meaning moderates the persuasive effect of physical actions: Buying, selling, touching, carrying, and cleaning thoughts as if they were commercial products. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 2, 460–471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & Tormala, Z. L. (2004). The self-validation of cognitive responses to advertisements. Journal of Consumer Research, 30, 559–573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Brownlow, S. (1992). Seeing is believing: Facial appearance, credibility, and attitude change. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 16(2), 101–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Burgoon, J. K. (1978). A communication model of personal space violations: Explication and an initial test. Human Communication Research, 4, 129–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Burgoon, J. K., Birk, T., & Pfau, M. (1990). Nonverbal behaviors, persuasion, and credibility. Human Communication Research, 17(1), 140–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Burgoon, J. K., & Dillman, L. (1995). Gender, immediacy, and nonverbal communication. In P. J. Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody (Eds.), Gender, power, and communication in human relationships (pp. 63–82). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  32. Burgoon, J. K., Dillman, L., & Stern, L. A. (1993). Adaptation in dyadic interaction: Defining and operationalizing patterns of reciprocity and compensation. Communication Theory, 3(4), 295–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Burgoon, J. K., & Dunbar, N. E. (2006). Nonverbal expressions of dominance and power in human relationships. In Valerie Manusov & Miles L. Patterson (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 279–298). Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications Inc.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Burgoon, J. K., Stern, L. A., & Dillman, L. (1995). Interpersonal adaptation: Dyadic interaction patterns. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 116–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Cappella, J. N., & Greene, J. O. (1982). A discrepancy-arousal explanation of mutual influence in expressive behavior for adult and infant-adult interaction. Communication Monographs, 49, 89–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Cashdan, E. (1998). Smiles, speech, and body posture: How women and men display sociometric status and power. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22(4), 209–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. P. Zanna, J. M. Olson, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3–39). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  39. Chaiken, S., & Maheswaran, D. (1994). Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: Effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 460–473.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Chaikin, A. (1978). Students’ reactions to teachers’ physical attractiveness and nonverbal behavior: Two exploratory studies. Psychology in the Schools, 15(4), 588–595.Google Scholar
  41. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Chartrand, T. L., & Lakin, J. L. (2013). The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 285–308.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Cialdini, R. (2001). Influence: Science and practice (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  44. Civile, C., & Obhl, S. S. (2017). Students wearing police uniforms exhibit biased attention toward individuals wearing hoodies. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 62.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  45. Clark, J. K., Wegener, D. T., Habashi, M. M., & Evans, A. T. (2012). Source expertise and persuasion: The effects of perceived opposition or support on message scrutiny. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(1), 90–100.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Crivelli, C., & Fridlund, A. J. (2018). Facial displays are tools for social influence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 388–399.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. DeBono, K. G., & Harnish, R. J. (1988). Source expertise, source attractiveness, and the processing of persuasive information: A functional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(4), 541–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Dipboye, R. L., Arvey, R. D., & Terpstra, D. E. (1977). Sex and physical attractiveness of raters and applicants as determinants of resume evaluations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 288–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In T. D. T. Power (Ed.), The handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 45–60). Sussex: Wiley.Google Scholar
  50. Evans, A. T. (2014). The impact of observed nonverbal cues on message-based persuasion. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa.Google Scholar
  51. Evans, A. T., & Clark, J. K. (2012). Source characteristics and persuasion: The role of self-monitoring in self-validation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 383–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Fabbri-Destro, M., & Rizzolatti, G. (2008). Mirror neurons and mirror systems in monkeys and humans. Physiology, 23(3), 171–179.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1013–1027.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Festinger, L., & Thibaut, J. (1951). Interpersonal communication in small groups. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46(1), 92–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior. Reading: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  57. Fleming, M. A., & Petty, R. E. (2000). Identity and persuasion: An elaboration likelihood approach. In D. J. Terry & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Attitudes, behavior, and social context: The role of norms and group membership (pp. 171–199). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  58. Forehand, M. R., & Perkins, A. (2005). Implicit assimilation and explicit contrast: A set/reset model of response to celebrity voice-overs. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(3), 435–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Galinsky, A. D., Rucker, D. D., & Magee, J. C. (2015). Power: Past findings, present considerations, and future directions. APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 421–460.Google Scholar
  60. Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(12), 493–501.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Greenwald, A. G., & Albert, R. D. (1968). Acceptance and recall of improvised arguments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 31–34.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Gruder, C. L., Cook, T. D., Hennigan, K. M., Flay, B. R., Alessis, C., & Halamaj, J. (1978). Empirical tests of the absolute sleeper effect predicted from the discounting cue hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1061–1074.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Guido, G., Peluso, A. M., & Moffa, V. (2014). Beardedness in advertising: Effects on endorsers’ credibility and purchase intentions. Journal of Marketing Communications, 17(1), 37–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Hale, J. L., & Burgoon, J. K. (1984). Models of reactions to changes in nonverbal immediacy. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 8(4), 287–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Hall, E. T. (1966). Hidden dimension. Garden City: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  67. Haugtvedt, C., Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1992). Need for cognition and advertising: Understanding the role of personality variables in consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 1(3), 239–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Haugtvedt, C., Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Steidley, T. (1988). Personality and ad effectiveness: Exploring the utility of need for cognition. Advances in Consumer Research, 15, 209–212.Google Scholar
  69. Heflick, N. A., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2014). Seeing eye to body: The literal objectification of women. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(3), 225–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Horcajo, J., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2014). Multiple roles for majority versus minority source status on persuasion when source status follows the message. Social Influence, 9, 37–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Hosman, L. A., Huebner, T. M., & Siltanen, S. A. (2002). The impact of power-of-speech style, argument strength, and need for cognition on impression formation, cognitive responses, and persuasion. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 21(4), 361–381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Hovland, C. I., Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H. (1953). Communication and persuasion: Psychological studies of opinion change. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  73. Jones, C. R., Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2009). Implicit misattribution as a mechanism underlying evaluative conditioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 933–948.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  74. Kang, Y. S., & Herr, P. M. (2006). Beauty and the beholder: Toward an integrative model of communication source effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(1), 123–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Kelman, H. C. (1958). Compliance, identification and internalization: Three processes of attitude change. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 2, 51–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Krumhuber, E. G., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2009). Can Duchenne smiles be feigned? New evidence on felt and false smiles. Emotion, 9(6), 807–820.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Kunecke, J., Wilhelm, O., & Sommer, W. (2017). Emotion recognition in nonverbal face-to-face communication. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 41(3), 221–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Langton, S. R. H. (2000). The mutual influence of gaze and head orientation in the analysis of social attention direction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 825–845.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Langton, S. R. H., Watt, R. J., & Bruce, V. (2000). Do the eyes have it? Cues to the direction of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 50–59.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Leder, H., Forster, M., & Gerger, G. (2011). The glasses stereotype revisited: Effects of eyeglasses on perception, recognition, and impression of faces. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 70(4), 211–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Locke, C. C., & Anderson, C. (2015). The downside of looking like a leader: Power, nonverbal confidence, and participative decision-making. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 58, 42–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Mann, T. C., & Ferguson, M. J. (2016). Reversing implicit first impressions through reinterpretation after a two-day delay. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 68, 122–127.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  83. McConnell, A. R., Rydell, R. J., Strain, L. M., & Mackie, D. M. (2008). Forming implicit and explicit attitudes toward individuals: Social group association cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(5), 792–807.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. McGuire, W. J. (1981). The probabilogical model of cognitive structure and attitude change. In R. E. Petty, T. M. Ostrom, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Cognitive responses in persuasion. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  85. Mello, J., Garcia-Marques, T., Briñol, P., Cancela, A., & Petty, R. E. (2017). The effect of self-objectification and perceived physical attractiveness on thought-reliance. In Presented at the 18th General Meeting of the European Association of Social Psychology. Granada, Spain.Google Scholar
  86. Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–739.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Niedenthal, P. M., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M., & Hess, U. (2010). The Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 417–480.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Okubo, M., Ishikawa, K., Kobayashi, A., & Suzuki, H. (2017). Can I trust you? Laterality of facial trustworthiness in an economic game. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 41, 21–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Ottati, V., Terkildsen, N., & Hubbard, C. (1997). Happy faces elicit heuristic processing in a televised impression formation task: A cognitive tuning account. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(11), 1144–1156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Pallak, S. R. (1983). Salience of a communicator’s physical attractiveness and persuasion: A heuristic versus systematic processing interpretation. Social Cognition, 2(2), 158–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Paredes, B., Stavraki, M., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2013). Smiling after thinking increases reliance on thoughts. Social Psychology, 44, 349–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Parkinson, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2015). Current emotion research in social psychology: Thinking about emotions and other people. Emotion Review, 7(4), 371–380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Patterson, M. L. (1976). An arousal model of interpersonal intimacy. Psychological Review, 83(3), 235–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Patterson, M. L. (1982). A sequential functional model of nonverbal behavior. Psychological Review, 89(3), 231–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Patterson, M. L. (1995). A parallel process model of nonverbal communication. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 19(1), 3–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Patterson, M. L. (2018). A systems model of dyadic nonverbal interaction. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-018-00292-w.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2010). Attitude structure and change: Implications for implicit measures. In B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 335–352). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  98. Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2012). The elaboration likelihood model. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 224–245). London: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., Tormala, Z. L., & Wegener, D. T. (2007). The role of meta-cognition in social judgment. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: A handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 254–284). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  100. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1979). Issue involvement can increase or decrease persuasion by enhancing message-relevant cognitive responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1915–1926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Issue involvement as a moderator of the effects on attitude of advertising content and context. Advances in Consumer Research, 8, 20–24.Google Scholar
  102. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1984). The effects of involvement on response to argument quantity and quality: Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 69–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Goldman, R. (1981a). Personal involvement as a determinant of argument–based persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 847–855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Heesacker, M. (1981b). The use of rhetorical questions in persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 432–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Schumann, D. (1983). Central and peripheral routes to advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer Research, 10, 135–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Petty, R. E., Harkins, S. G., & Williams, K. D. (1980). The effects of group diffusion of cognitive effort on attitudes: An information processing view. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 81–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Petty, R. E., & Krosnick, J. A. (Eds.). (1995). Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Mahwah: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  109. Petty, R. E., Ostrom, T. M., & Brock, T. C. (1981c). Historical foundations of the cognitive response approach to attitudes and persuasion. In R. Petty, T. Ostrom, & T. Brock (Eds.), Cognitive responses in persuasion (pp. 5–29). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  110. Petty, R. E., Schumann, D. W., Richman, S. A., & Strathman, A. J. (1993). Positive mood and persuasion: Different roles for affect under high and low elaboration conditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 5–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Puckett, J. M., Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Fisher, D. L. (1983). The relative impact of age and attractiveness stereotypes on persuasion. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 340–343.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  112. Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 364–382.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  113. Rizzolatti, G. (2005). The mirror neuron system and its function in humans. Anatomy and Embryology, 210(5), 419–421.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  114. Rubenstein, A. J., Langlois, J. H., & Roggman, L. A. (2002). What makes a face attractive and why: The role of averageness in defining facial beauty. In G. Rhodes & L. A. Zebrowitz (Eds.), Advances in visual cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 1–33), Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary, cognitive, and social perspectives. Westport: Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
  115. Rucker, D. D., Tormala, Z. L., Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2014). Consumer conviction and commitment: An appraisal-based framework for attitude certainty. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(1), 119–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Rychlowska, M., Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., Schyns, P. G., Martin, J. D., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2017). Functional smiles: Tools for love, sympathy, and war. Psychological Science, 28(9), 1259–1270.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  117. Rydell, R. J., & McConnell, A. R. (2006). Understanding implicit and explicit attitude change: A systems of reasoning analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6), 995–1008.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Schubert, T. W., Waldzus, S., & Seibt, B. (2008). The embodiment of power and communalism in space and bodily contact. In G. R. Semin & E. R. Smith (Eds.), Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 160–183). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Shavitt, S., Swan, S., Lowrey, T. M., & Wanke, M. (1994). The interaction of endorser attractiveness and involvement in persuasion depends on the goal that guides message processing. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 3(2), 137–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. Sherif, M. (1936). The psychology of social norms. Oxford: Harper.Google Scholar
  122. Shteynberg, G. (2015). Shared attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 579–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. Shteynberg, G., Bramlett, J. M., Fles, E. H., & Cameron, J. (2016). The broadcast of shared attention and its impact on political persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 665–673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. Smith, C. T., DeHouwer, J. D., & Nosek, B. A. (2012). Consider the source: Persuasion of implicit evaluations is moderated by source credibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 193–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Snyder, M. (1979). Self-monitoring processes. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 85–128). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  126. Staats, A. W., & Staats, C. K. (1958). Attitudes established by classical conditioning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 57, 37–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  127. Tanner, R., & Chartrand, T. (2006). The convincing Chameleon: The impact of mimicry on persuasion. Advances in Consumer Research, 33, 409–412.Google Scholar
  128. Tiedens, L. Z., & Fragale, A. R. (2003). Power moves: Complementarity in dominant and submissive nonverbal behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(3), 558–568.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  129. Tormala, Z. L., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2006). When credibility attacks: The reverse impact of source credibility on persuasion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 684–691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  130. Tormala, Z. L., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2007). Multiple roles for source credibility under high elaboration: It’s all in the timing. Social Cognition, 25, 536–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Toscano, H., Schubert, T. W., Dotsch, R., Falvello, V., & Todorov, A. (2016). Physical strength as a cue to dominance: A data-driven approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(12), 1603–1616.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Van Kleef, G. A., van den Berg, H., & Heerdink, M. W. (2015). The persuasive power of emotions: Effects of emotional expressions on attitude formation and change. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(4), 1124–1142.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  133. Vogel, T., Kutzner, F., Fiedler, K., & Freytag, P. (2010). Exploiting attractiveness in persuasion: Senders’ implicit theories about receivers’ processing motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(6), 830–842.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  134. Watkins, L. M., & Johnston, L. (2000). Screening job applicants: The impact of physical attractiveness and application quality. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 8(2), 76–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  135. Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Flexible correction processes in social judgement: The role of naive theories in corrections for perceived bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 36–51.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  136. Yee, N., Bailenson, J. N., & Ducheneaut, N. (2009). The proteus effect: Implications of transformed digital self-representation on online and offline behavior. Communication Research, 36(2), 285–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  137. Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  138. Zebrowitz, L. A., & Montepare, J. M. (2008). Social psychological face perception: Why appearance matters. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(3), 1497–1517.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  139. Ziegler, R., von Schwichow, A., & Diehl, M. (2005). Matching the message source to attitude functions: Implications for biased processing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 645–653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Joshua J. Guyer
    • 1
    Email author
  • Pablo Briñol
    • 1
  • Richard E. Petty
    • 2
  • Javier Horcajo
    • 1
  1. 1.Facultad de PsicologíaUniversidad Autónoma de MadridMadridSpain
  2. 2.Ohio State UniversityColumbusUSA

Personalised recommendations