Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

, Volume 38, Issue 1, pp 13–29 | Cite as

The Expression of Victory and Loss: Estimating Who’s Leading or Trailing from Nonverbal Cues in Sports

Original Paper

Abstract

The goal of the paper was to test if humans can detect whether athletes are trailing or leading in sports based on the perception of thin slices of athletes’ nonverbal behavior. In Experiment 1, participants who were unexperienced in the respective sports watched short videos depicting basketball and table tennis players and rated whether athletes were trailing or leading. Results indicated that participants could significantly differentiate between trailing and leading athletes in both team and individual sports. Experiment 2 showed that children were also able to distinguish between trailing and leading athletes based on nonverbal behavior. Comparison with the adult results from Experiment 1 revealed that the adult ratings corresponded to a higher degree with the actual scores during the game compared to the children’s. In Experiment 3, we replicated the findings from Experiment 1 with both expert and unexperienced participants and a different set of stimuli from team handball. Both experts and unexpert participants were able to differentiate between leading and trailing athletes. Our findings are in line with evolutionary accounts of nonverbal behavior and suggest that humans display nonverbal signals as a consequence of leading or trailing which are reliably interpreted by others. By comparing this effect as a function of different age groups we provide evidence that although even young children can differentiate between leading and trailing athletes, the decoding of subtle nonverbal cues continues to develop with increasing experience and maturation processes.

Keywords

Sport Emotion expression Nonverbal behavior Thin slices Development 

References

  1. Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A. (2000). Toward a histology of social behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices of the behavioral stream. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 32, pp. 201–271). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  2. Ambady, N., & Gray, H. M. (2002). On being sad and mistaken: Mood effects on the accuracy of thin-slice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 947–961. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.83.4.947.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ambady, N., Krabbenhoft, M. A., & Hogan, D. (2006). The 30-sec sale: Using thin-slice judgments to evaluate sales effectiveness. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16, 4–13. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1601_2.Google Scholar
  4. Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 236–274. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.111.2.256.Google Scholar
  5. Archer, J. (2006). Testosterone and human aggression: An evaluation of the challenge hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 319–345. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.12.007.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Balas, B., Kanwisher, N., & Saxe, R. (2012). Thin-slice perception develops slowly. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 257–264. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.01.002.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Batty, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2006). The development of emotional face processing during childhood. Developmental Science, 9, 207–220. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00480.x.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Booth, A., Shelley, G., Mazur, A., Tharp, G., & Kittok, R. (1989). Testosterone and winning and losing in human competition. Hormones and Behavior, 23, 556–571. doi:10.1016/0018-506X(89)90042-1.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Borkenau, P., Mauer, N., Riemann, R., Spinath, F. M., & Angleitner, A. (2004). Thin slices of behavior as cues of personality and intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 599–614. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.4.599.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Burgoon, J. K. (1996). Nonverbal signals. In M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal communication (2nd ed., pp. 344–390). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  11. Carney, D. R., Colvin, C. R., & Hall, J. A. (2007). A thin slice perspective on the accuracy of first impressions. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(5), 1054–1072. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.01.004.Google Scholar
  12. Carney, D., Cuddy, A., & Yap, A. (2010). Power posing: Brief nonverbal displays affect neuroendocrine levels and risk tolerance. Psychological Science, 21, 1363–1368. doi:10.1177/0956797610383437.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Clark-Carter, D. (1997). Doing quantitative psychological research: From design to report. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  14. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  15. Cumming, G. (2012). Understanding the new statistics: Effect sizes, confidence intervals, and meta-analysis. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  16. Curhan, J., & Pentland, A. (2007). Thin slices of negotiation: Predicting outcomes from conversational dynamics within the first five minutes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 802–811. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.3.8027.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of emotions in man and animals. London: Murray.Google Scholar
  18. de Waal, F. (1998). Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Durand, K., Gallay, M., Seigneuric, A., Robichon, F., & Baudouin, J. Y. (2007). The development of facial emotion recognition: The role of configural information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 14–27. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2006.12.001.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169–200. doi:10.1080/02699939208411068.Google Scholar
  21. Fiedler, K. (2011). Voodoo correlations are everywhere—not only in neuroscience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 163–171. doi:10.1177/1745691611400237.Google Scholar
  22. Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. San Diego, CA: Academic.Google Scholar
  23. Furley, P., & Dicks, M. (2012). “Hold your head high”. The influence of emotional versus neutral nonverbal expressions of dominance and submissiveness in baseball. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 43, 294–311.Google Scholar
  24. Furley, P., Dicks, M., & Memmert, D. (2012a). Nonverbal behavior in soccer: The influence of dominant and submissive body language on the impression formation and expectancy of success of soccer players. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 34, 61–82.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Furley, P., Dicks, M., Stendtke, F., & Memmert, D. (2012b). “Get it out the way. The wait’s killing me.” Hastening and hiding during soccer penalty kicks. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13, 454–465. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.01.009.Google Scholar
  26. Gao, X., & Maurer, D. (2010). A happy story: Developmental changes in children’s sensitivity to facial expressions of varying intensities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 67–86. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.05.003.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Greenlees, I. A., Bradley, A., Holder, T. P., & Thelwell, R. C. (2005a). The impact of opponents’ non-verbal behaviour on the first impressions and outcome expectations of table-tennis players. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 6, 103–115. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2003.10.002.Google Scholar
  28. Greenlees, I. A., Buscombe, R., Thelwell, R. C., Holder, T. P., & Rimmer, M. (2005b). Impact of opponents’ clothing and body language on impression formation and outcome expectations. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 27, 39–52.Google Scholar
  29. Kolb, B., Wilson, B., & Taylor, L. (1992). Developmental changes in the recognition and comprehension of facial expression: Implications for frontal lobe function. Brain and Cognition, 20, 74–84. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(92)90062-Q.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). The psychology of being “right”. The problem of accuracy in social perception and cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 394–409. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.106.3.395.Google Scholar
  31. Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2006). The thrill of victory and the agony of defeat: Spontaneous expressions of medal winners at the 2004 Athens Olympic Games. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 568–581. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.568.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 353–397. doi:10.1017/S0140525X98001228.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. McArthur, L. Z., & Baron, R. M. (1983). Toward an ecological theory of social perception. Psychological Review, 90, 215–238. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.90.3.215.Google Scholar
  34. Mehta, P. H., Jones, A. C., & Josephs, R. A. (2008). The social endocrinology of dominance: Basal testosterone predicts cortisol changes and behavior following victory and defeat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 1078–1093. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.6.1078.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Mehta, P. H., & Josephs, R. A. (2010). Testosterone and cortisol jointly regulate dominance: Evidence for a dual-hormone hypothesis. Hormones and Behavior, 58, 898–906. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.08.020.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Parkinson, B. (2005). Do facial movements express emotions or communicate motives? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 278–311. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0904_1.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Pashler, H., & Wagenmarkers, E.-J. (2012). Editors’ introduction to the special section on replicability in psychological science. A crisis of confidence? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 528–530. doi:10.1177/1745691612465253.Google Scholar
  38. Ross, P. D., Polson, L., & Grosbras, M. H. (2012). Developmental changes in emotion recognition from full-light and point-light displays of body movement. PLoS One, 7(9), e44815. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0044815.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2008). The face of success: Inferences from chief executive officers’ appearance predict company profits. Psychological Science, 19, 109–111. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02054.x.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Shariff, A. F., & Tracy, J. L. (2011). What are emotion expressions for? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 395–399. doi:10.1177/0963721411424739.Google Scholar
  41. Thomas, L. A., De Bellis, M. D., Graham, R., & LaBar, K. S. (2007). Development of emotional facial recognition in late childhood and adolescence. Developmental Science, 10, 547–558. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00614.x.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Thomas, K. M., Drevets, W. C., Whalen, P. J., Eccard, C. H., Dahl, R. E., et al. (2001). Amygdala response to facial expressions in children and adults. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 309–316. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01066-0.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Tonks, J., Williams, W. H., Frampton, I., Yates, P., & Slater, A. (2007). Assessing emotion recognition in 9–15-years olds: preliminary analysis of abilities in reading emotion from faces, voices and eyes. Brain Injury, 21, 623–629. doi:10.1080/02699050701426865.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Tracy, J. L., & Matsumoto, D. (2008). The spontaneous display of pride and shame: Evidence for biologically innate nonverbal displays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 11655–11660. doi:10.1073/pnas.0802686105.Google Scholar
  45. Yong, E. (2012). Replication studies: Bad copy. Nature, 485, 298–300. doi:10.1038/485298a.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute of Cognitive and Team/Racket Sport ResearchGerman Sport University CologneCologneGermany
  2. 2.University of HeidelbergHeidelbergGermany

Personalised recommendations