Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

, Volume 34, Issue 3, pp 155–167 | Cite as

Vocal and Physiological Changes in Response to the Physical Attractiveness of Conversational Partners

  • Susan M. Hughes
  • Sally D. Farley
  • Bradley C. Rhodes
Original Paper

Abstract

We examined how individuals may change their voices when speaking to attractive versus unattractive individuals, and if it were possible for others to perceive these vocal changes. In addition, we examined if any concurrent physiological effects occurred when speaking with individuals who varied in physical attractiveness. We found that both sexes used a lower-pitched voice and showed a higher level of physiological arousal when speaking to the more attractive, opposite-sex target. Furthermore, independent raters evaluated the voice samples directed toward the attractive target (versus the unattractive target) as sounding more pleasant when the two voice samples from the same person presented had a reasonably perceptually noticeable difference in pitch. These findings may have implications for the role voice plays in mate selection and attraction.

Keywords

Vocal changes Attractiveness Voice Physiological responses Romantic attraction 

References

  1. Amir, O., Biron-Shental, T., & Shabtai, E. (2006). Birth control pills and nonprofessional voice: Acoustic analyses. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 49(5), 1114–1126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anolli, L., & Ciceri, R. (2002). Analysis of the vocal profiles of male seduction: From exhibition to self-disclosure. Journal of General Psychology, 129, 149–169.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Apicella, C. L., Feinberg, D. R., & Marlowe, F. W. (2007). Voice pitch predicts reproductive success in male hunter-gatherers. Biology Letters, 3, 682–684.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D. J., Strong, G., Li, H., & Brown, L. L. (2005). Reward, motivation, and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense romantic love. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 327–337.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love. NeuroReport, 11, 3829–3834.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Berry, D. (1990). Vocal attractiveness and vocal babyishness: Effects on stranger, self and friend impressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 14, 141–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Collins, S. A. (2000). Men’s voices and women’s choices. Animal Behaviour, 60, 773–780.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Collins, S. A., & Missing, C. (2003). Vocal and visual attractiveness are related in women. Animal Behaviour, 65, 997–1004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Daniel, H. J., & McCabe, R. B. (1992). Gender differences in the perception of vocal sexiness. In J. M. G. van der Dennen (Ed.), The nature of the sexes: The sociobiology of sex differences and the ‘‘battle of the sexes’’ (p. 55). The Netherlands: Origin Press.Google Scholar
  10. Davitz, J. R. (1964). The communication of emotional meaning. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
  11. Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Scherer, K. (1976). Body movements and voice pitch in deceptive interaction. Semiotica, 16, 23–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Ethofer, T., Wiethoff, S., Anders, S., Kreifelts, B., Grod, W., & Wildgruber, D. (2007). The voices of seduction: Cross-gender effects in the processing of erotic prosody. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 334–337.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Evans, S., Neave, N., & Wakelin, D. (2006). Relationships between vocal characteristics and body size and shape in human males: An evolutionary explanation for a deep male voice. Biological Psychology, 72, 160–163.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2008). The role of femininity and averageness of voice pitch in aesthetic judgments of women’s voices. Perception, 37, 615–623.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Manipulation of fundamental and formant frequencies influence the attractiveness of human male voices. Animal Behavior, 69, 561–568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hall, J. A., & Braunwald, K. G. (1981). Gender cues in conversations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 99–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hess, E. H., & Polt, J. M. (1960). Pupil size as related to interest value of visual stimuli. Science, 132, 349–350.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Hughes, S. M., Dispenza, F., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2004). Ratings of voice attractiveness predict sexual behavior and body configuration. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 295–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hughes, S. M., Harrison, M. A., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2002). The sound of symmetry: Voice as a marker of developmental instability. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 173–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Istvan, J., Griffitt, W., & Weidner, G. (1982). Sexual arousal and the polarization of perceived sexual attractiveness. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 4, 307–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Jones, B. C., Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., & Vukovic, J. (2008). Integrating cues of social interest and pitch in men’s preferences for women’s voices. Biology Letters, 4, 192–194.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Karpf, A. (2006). The human voice. New York, NY: Bloomsbury Publishing.Google Scholar
  23. Kimble, C. E., & Seidel, S. D. (1991). Vocal signs of confidence. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, 99–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kramer, E. R. (1963). Judgment of personal characteristics and emotions from nonverbal properties of speech. Psychological Bulletin, 60, 408–420.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Kramer, E. R. (1964). Elimination of cues in judgments of emotion from voice. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 390–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Ladefoged, P. (1996). Elements of acoustic phonetics (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  27. Leaderbrand, K., Dekam, J., Morey, A., & Tuma, L (2008). The effects of voice pitch on perceptions of attractiveness: Do you sound hot or not. Winona State University Psychology Student Journal. Google Scholar
  28. Montepare, J. M., & Vega, C. (1988). Women’s vocal reactions to intimate and casual male friends. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 103–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Oguchi, T., & Kikuchi, H. (1997). Voice and interpersonal attraction. Japanese Psychological Research, 39, 56–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Oksenberg, L., Coleman, L., & Cannell, C. F. (1986). Interviewers’ voice and refusal rates in telephone surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 50, 97–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Pemberton, C., McCormack, P., & Russell, A. (1998). Have women’s voices lowered across time? A cross-sectional study of Australian women’s voices. Journal of Voice, 12, 208–213.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Pipitone, R. N., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2008). Women’s attractiveness varies across the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 268–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Puts, D. A. (2005). Mating context and menstrual phase affect women’s preferences for male voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 388–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Puts, D. A., Gaulin, J. C., & Verdolini, K. (2006). Dominance of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 283–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Riding, D., Lonsdale, D., & Brown, B. (2006). The effects of average fundamental frequency and variance of fundamental frequency on male vocal attractiveness to women. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30, 55–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Rousey, C. L., & Moriarty, A. E. (1965). Diagnostic implications of speech sounds: The reflections of developmental conflict and trauma. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
  37. Scherer, K. R. (1986). Vocal affect expression: A review and a model for future research. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 143–165.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, E. (1977). Social perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of Social stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 656–666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Steckler, N. A., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Sex differences in nonverbal and verbal communication with bosses, peers, and subordinates. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 157–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Stern, R. M., Botto, R. W., & Herrick, C. D. (1972). Behavioral and physiological effects of false heart rate feedback: A replication and extension. Psychophysiology, 9, 21–29.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Streeter, L. A., Krauss, R. M., Geller, V. J., Olson, C. T., & Apple, W. (1977). Pitch changes during attempted deception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 345–350.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Taylor, S. F., Liberzon, I., & Koeppe, R. A. (2000). The effect of graded aversive stimuli on limbic and visual activation. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1415–1425.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Tuomi, S. K., & Fisher, J. E. (1979). Characteristics of a simulated sexy voice. Folia Phoniatrica, 31, 242–249.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Uchiyama, I., Hanari, T., Ito, T., & Takahashi, K. (1990). Patterns of psychological and physiological responses for common affects elicited by films. Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 33, 36–41.Google Scholar
  45. Valins, S. (1967). Emotionality and information concerning internal reactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 458–463.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Van Bezooijen, R. (1995). Sociocultural aspects of pitch differences between Japanese and Dutch women. Language and Speech, 38, 253–265.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Vukovic, J., Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Welling, L. L. M., Little, A. C., et al. (2008). Self-rated attractiveness predicts individual differences in women’s preferences for masculine male voices. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 451–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Winton, W. N., Putnam, L. E., & Krauss, R. M. (1984). Facial and autonomic manifestations of the dimensional structure of emotion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 195–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Woll, S. B., & McFall, M. E. (1979). The effects of false feedback on attributed arousal and rated attractiveness in female subjects. Journal of Personality, 47, 214–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Zuckerman, M., & Driver, R. (1989). What sounds beautiful is good: The vocal attractiveness stereotype. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 67–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Susan M. Hughes
    • 1
  • Sally D. Farley
    • 2
  • Bradley C. Rhodes
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyAlbright CollegeReadingUSA
  2. 2.University of BaltimoreBaltimoreUSA

Personalised recommendations