Advertisement

Journal of Happiness Studies

, Volume 13, Issue 6, pp 985–998 | Cite as

Overall Hedonic Evaluations and Evaluation of Specific Moments from Past Relationships and High School Days

  • Simon KempEmail author
  • Zhe ChenEmail author
Research Paper
  • 260 Downloads

Abstract

Two studies were conducted in which student participants gave overall hedonic evaluations of past relationships or their high school days, and, additionally, gave similar evaluations of the best, most memorable, worst, initial, and final moments from them. They also evaluated a typical moment and the percentages of good and bad times in these experiences. Such results from some individuals were given to other participants, who were also asked to make overall evaluations. Overall, the overall evaluations of the original participant related weakly rather than strongly to the evaluations of the specific moments. Generally, the other participants’ overall evaluations converged on those of the original recalling participants, but their overall evaluations of the past relationships were more positive than either their own initial evaluations or those of the original, recalling respondents. In sum, overall evaluations of these long, diverse experiences were not well predicted from valuations of the specific moments we investigated. Hedonic evaluations of past experiences may be influenced by appraisals of their success.

Keywords

Hedonic evaluation Peak-end Autobiographical memory 

References

  1. Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2000). When does duration matter in judgment and decision making? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 508–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ariely, D., & Zauberman, G. (2000). On the making of an experience: The effects of breaking and combining experiences on their overall evaluation. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13, 219–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Brewer, W. F. (1988). Memory for randomly sampled autobiographical events. In U. Neisser & E. Winograd (Eds.), Remembering reconsidered: Ecological and traditional approaches to memory (pp. 21–90). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Burt, C. D. B. (1992). Reconstruction of the duration of autobiographical events. Memory & Cognition, 20, 124–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Burt, C. D. B., Strongman, K. T., & Constanzo, C. L. (1998). Memorial distortions and homesickness following relocation. Australian Journal of Psychology, 50, 106–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Churchill, W. S. (1930). My early life: A roving commission. New York: Charles Scribner.Google Scholar
  7. Cojuharenco, I. (2007). Lay intuitions about overall evaluations of experiences. Judgment and Decision Making, 2, 40–47.Google Scholar
  8. Cojuharenco, I., & Ryvkin, D. (2007). Evaluations of experiences lived across time: Reconciling empirical findings with folk beliefs (Unpublished manuscript).Google Scholar
  9. Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 594–628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fredrickson, B., & Kahneman, D. (1993). Duration neglect in retrospective evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 45–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Kemp, S., & Burt, C. D. B. (2006). Memories of uncertain origin: Dreamt or real? Memory, 14, 87–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Kemp, S., Burt, C. D. B., & Furneaux, L. (2008). A test of the peak-end rule with extended autobiographical events. Memory & Cognition, 36, 132–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Kemp, S., Burt, C. D. B., & Sheen, M. (2003). Remembering dreamt and actual experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 577–591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Neisser, U. (1981). John Dean’s memory: A case study. Cognition, 9, 102–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Redelmeier, D. A., & Kahneman, D. (1996). Patients’ memories of painful medical treatments: Real-time and retrospective evaluations of two minimally invasive procedures. Pain, 66, 3–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Rubin, D. C. (Ed.). (1995). Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Rubin, D. C., Schrauf, R. W., & Greenberg, D. L. (2003). Remembering, reliving, and believing autobiographical memories: Inter- and intra-individual analyses. Memory & Cognition, 31, 887–901.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Schreiber, C. A., & Kahneman, D. (2000). Determinants of the remembered utility of aversive sounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 27–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Sheen, M., Kemp, S., & Rubin, D. (2001). Twins dispute memory ownership: A new false memory phenomenon. Memory & Cognition, 29, 779–788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Teasdale, J. D., Taylor, R., & Fogarty, S. J. (1980). Effects of induced elation-depression on the accessibility of memories of happy and unhappy experiences. Behavior Research and Therapy, 18, 339–446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Thompson, C. P. (1982). Memory for unique personal events: The roommate study. Memory & Cognition, 10, 324–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Walker, W. R., Skowronski, J. J., & Thompson, C. P. (2003). Life is pleasant—and memory helps keep it that way. Review of General Psychology, 9, 203–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Walker, W. R., Vogl, R. J., & Thompson, C. P. (1997). Autobiographical memory: Unpleasantness fades faster than pleasantness over time. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 399–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Psychology DepartmentUniversity of CanterburyChristchurchNew Zealand

Personalised recommendations