Advertisement

Journal of Happiness Studies

, Volume 11, Issue 4, pp 477–495 | Cite as

On Lions and Adolescents: Affective Temperaments and the Influence of Negative Stimuli on Memory

  • Danilo Garcia
  • Patricia Rosenberg
  • Arvid Erlandsson
  • Anver Siddiqui
Research Paper

Abstract

The present study investigated the relation between reaction to negative stimuli and memory for stimuli. The relation was further investigated using as a framework individuals’ affective temperaments (AFTs). Eighty adolescents participated in the study. The AFTs are based on selfreported affect and categorizes individuals in four temperaments: self-actualizing, high affective, low affective and self-destructive. Reaction to negative stimuli was measured by interpretation of specific words in a short story. Two days later, participants were presented with a list of words and asked which of them were present in the short story. Individuals’ AFTs were expected to predict the promotion of pleasure or the prevention of displeasure. On a general level, reaction to negative stimuli predicted memory for negative, positive and neutral words. At an individual level, self-actualizers and high affectives’ negative reaction predicted the memory of positive words (i.e., promotion). In contrast, low affectives’ negative and positive reaction predicted the memory of neutral words (i.e., prevention).

Keywords

Affective temperament Attention memory Negativity bias Positivity bias Prevention focus Promotion focus 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank the participants for their help facilitating the study. Appreciation is also directed to reviewers who helped improve the manuscript.

References

  1. Ash, C., & Huebner, E. S. (2001). Environmental events and life satisfaction reports of adolescents: A test of cognitive mediation. School Psychology International, 22, 320–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersection of emotion and cognition: Aging and the positivity effect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 117–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Caspi, A. (2000). The child is father of the man: Personality continuities from childhood to adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 158–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Charles, S. T., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Focusing on the positive: Age differences in memory for positive, negative, and neutral stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 85, 163–178.Google Scholar
  5. Cloninger, C. R. (2004). Feeling good—the science of well-being. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Cloninger, C. R. (2006). Fostering spirituality and well-being in clinical practice. Psychiatric Annals, 36, 1–6.Google Scholar
  7. Coelho, P. (2002). The alchemist [in Swedish]. Stockholm: Bazar.Google Scholar
  8. Corneille, O., Vermeulen, N., Luminet, O., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2003). A closer examination of the Asymmetrical Lexical-Affective Dissociation Hypothesis. Google Scholar
  9. Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for spinoza: Joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain [in Swedish]. Stockholm: Natur och Kultur.Google Scholar
  10. Dattilio, F. M. (2002). Homework assignments in couple and family therapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 535–547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Diener, E. (2008). Myths in the science of happiness, and directions for future research. In M. Eid & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 493–514). New York: The Guilford.Google Scholar
  13. Diener, E., Colvin, C. R., Pavot, W. G., & Allman, A. (1991a). The psychic costs of intensive positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 492–503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1996). Most people are happy. Psychological Science, 7, 181–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Diener, E., Sandvik, E., & Pavot, W. (1991b). Happiness is the frequency, not the intensity, of positive versus negative affect. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective well-being: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 119–139). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  16. Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2003). On wildebeests and humans: The preferential detection of negative stimuli. Psychological Science, 14, 14–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Dijksterhuis, A., Corneille, O., Aarts, H., Vermeulen, N., & Luminet, O. (2004). Yes, there is preferential detection of negative stimuli: A response to Labiouse. Psychological Science, 15, 571–572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dunst, C. J., & Lingerfelt, B. (1985). Maternal ratings of temperament and operant learning in two- to three-month-old infants. Child Development, 56, 555–563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  20. Faddegon, K., Scheeoers, D., & Ellemers, N. (2008). If we have the will, there will be a way: Regulatory focus as a group identity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 880–895.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fielder, K. (2001). Affective influences on social information processing. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 163–183). New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  22. Fogle, L. M., Huebner, E. S., & Laughlin, J. E. (2002). The relationship between temperament and life satisfaction in early adolescence: Cognitive and behavioral mediation models. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3, 373–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Garcia, D., & Siddiqui, A. (2009a). Adolescents’ affective temperaments: Life satisfaction, interpretation and memory of events. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 155–167. doi: 10.1080/17439760802399349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Garcia, D., & Siddiqui, A. (2009b). Adolescents’ psychological well-being and memory for life events: Influences on life satisfaction with respect to temperamental dispositions. Journal of Happiness Studies, 10, 387–503. doi: 10.1007/s10902-008-9096-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gunderson, J. G., Triebwasser, J., Phillips, K. A., & Sullivan, C. N. (1999). Personality and vulnerability to affective disorders. In C. R. Cloninger (Ed.), Personality and psychopathology (pp. 3–32). New York: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
  26. Haidt, J. (2006). The happiness hypothesis: Finding modern truth in ancient wisdom. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  27. Harter, S., Marold, D. B., Whitesell, N. R., & Cobbs, G. (1996). A model of the effects of perceived parent and peer support on adolescent false self behavior. Child Development, 67, 360–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Higgins, E. T. (2001). Promotion and prevention experiences: Relating emotions to nonemotional motivational states. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 50–74). New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  30. Higgins, E. T., Shah, J., & Friedman, R. (1997). Emotional responses to goal attainment: Strength of regulatory focus as moderator. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 72, 515–525.Google Scholar
  31. Huebner, E. S., & Diener, C. (2008). Research on life satisfaction of children and youth: Implications fort he delivery of school-related services. In M. Eid & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 376–392). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  32. Idson, L. C., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2000). Distinguishing gains from non-losses and losses from non-gains: A regulatory focus perspective on hedonic intensity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 252–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Jermann, F., Van der Linden, M., Laurençon, M., & Schmitt, B. (2009). Recollective experience during recognition of emotional words in clinical depression. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 31, 27–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kinzler, K. D., & Shutts, K. (2008). Memory for “mean” over “nice”: The influence of threat on children’s face memory. Cognition, 107, 775–783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, emotion, and well-being: Good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cognition & Emotion, 1, 93–125.Google Scholar
  36. Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1989). Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative mood induction procedures. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 21–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Larson, R. W. (2000). Toward a psychology of positive youth development. American Psychologist, 55, 170–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Leight, K. A., & Ellis, H. C. (1981). Emotional mood states, strategies, and state dependency in memory. Journal of verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 251–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Lockhart, K. L., Chang, B., & Story, T. (2002). Young children’s beliefs about the stability of traits: Protective optimism? Child Development, 73, 1408–1430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 6, 803–855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Maccoby, E. E. (2000). Parenting and its effects on children: On reading and misreading behavior genetics. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Martin, K. M., & Huebner, E. S. (2007). Peer victimization and prosocial experiences and emotional well-being of middle school students. Psychology in the Schools, 44, 199–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5, 100–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. McCullough, G., Huebner, E. S., & Laughlin, J. M. (2000). Life events, self-concept, and adolescents’ positive subjective well-being. Psychology in the Schools, 37, 281–290.Google Scholar
  46. Norlander, T., Bood, S.-Å., & Archer, T. (2002). Performance during stress: Affective personality age, and regularity of physical exercise. Social Behavior and Personality, 30, 495–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Norlander, T., Johansson, Å., & Bood, S.-Å. (2005). The affective personality: Its relation to quality of sleep, well-being and stress. Social Behavior and Personality, 33, 709–722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Pury, C. L. S. (2004). Low positive affect and less extreme emotional encoding. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 149–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Rigby, B. T., & Huebner, E. S. (2004). Do causal attributions mediate the relationship between personality characteristics and life satisfaction in adolescence? Psychology in the Schools, 42, 91–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Schimmack, U. (2007). Methodological issues in the assessment of the affective component of subjective well-being. In A. D. Ong & M. H. M. Van Dulmen (Eds.), Oxford handbook of methods in positive psychology (pp. 96–110). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  52. Schimmack, U., Bockenholt, U., & Reisenzein, R. (2002). Response styles in affect ratings: Making a mountain out of a molehill. Journal of Personality Assessment, 78, 461–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Schimmack, U., & Diener, E. (1997). Affect intensity: Separating intensity and frequency in repeatedly measured affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1313–1330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Seidlitz, L., & Diener, E. (1993). Memory for positive versus negative life events: Theories for the differences between happy and unhappy persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 654–664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Shah, J., & Higgins, E. T. (2001). Regulatory concerns and appraisal efficiency: The general impact of promotion and prevention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 693–705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Sheffield Morris, A. (2003). Adolescents’ emotion regulation in daily life: Links to depressive symptoms and problem behavior. Child Development, 74, 1869–1880.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2001). Adolescent development. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 83–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Tellegen, A. (1993). Folk concepts and psychological concept of personality and personality disorder. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 122–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Touryan, S. R., Johnson, M. K., Mitchell, K. J., Farb, N., Cunningham, W. A., & Raye, C. L. (2007). The influence of self-regulatory focus on encoding of, and memory for, emotional words. Social Neuroscience, 2, 14–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 320–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Walker, W. R., Skowronski, J. J., & Thompson, C. P. (2003). Life is pleasant–and memory helps to keep it that way! Review of General Psychology, 7, 203–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Watkins, P. C., Martin, C. K., & Stern, L. D. (2000). Unconscious memory bias in depression: Perceptual and conceptual processes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 282–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-X: Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule–expanded form. Iowa: University of Iowa Press.Google Scholar
  64. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Wentura, D., Rothermund, K., & Bak, P. (2000). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of approach- and avoidance-related social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1024–1037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Williams, J. M. G., Matthews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Williams, M. A., & Mattingley, J. B. (2006). Do angry men get noticed? Current Biology, 16, R402–R404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2001). From chump to champ: People’s appraisals of their earlier and present selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 572–584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Xing, C., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). Aiming at happiness: How motivation affects attention to and memory for emotional images. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 249–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Danilo Garcia
    • 1
  • Patricia Rosenberg
    • 2
  • Arvid Erlandsson
    • 3
  • Anver Siddiqui
    • 4
    • 5
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyKarlskrona UniversityKarlstadSweden
  2. 2.Galären EducationsKarlskronaSweden
  3. 3.International Christian UniversityMitaka, TokyoJapan
  4. 4.Växjö UniversityVaxjoSweden
  5. 5.Umeå UniversityUmeaSweden

Personalised recommendations