Skip to main content
Log in

Reduced Risk-Taking After Prior Losses in Pathological Gamblers Under Treatment and Healthy Control Group but not in Problem Gamblers

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Gambling Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A group of pathological gamblers and a group of problem gamblers (i.e., gamblers at risk of becoming pathological) were compared to healthy controls on their risk-taking propensity after prior losses. Each participant played both the Balloon Analogue Risk Taking task (BART) and a modified version of the same task, where individuals face five repeated predetermined early losses at the onset of the game. No significant difference in risk-taking was found between groups on the standard BART task, while significant differences emerged when comparing behaviors in the two tasks: both pathological gamblers and controls reduced their risk-taking tendency after prior losses in the modified BART compared to the standard BART, whereas problem gamblers showed no reduction in risk-taking after prior losses. We interpret these results as a sign of a reduced sensitivity to negative feedback in problem gamblers which might contribute to explain their loss-chasing tendency.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed) (DSM-5). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.) TR., 280. doi:10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.744053.

  • APA. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., pp. 69–81). Washington: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barratt, E. (1959). Anxiety and impulsiveness related to psychomotor efficiency. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 9(3), 191–198. doi:10.2466/pms.1959.9.3.191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1–3), 7–15. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, A. M., & Robbins, S. J. (2007). The role of framing effects in performance on the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Personality and Individual Differences, 43(2), 221–230. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bibby, P. A. (2016). Loss-chasing, alexithymia, and impulsivity in a gambling task: Alexithymia as a precursor to loss-chasing behavior when gambling. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(3), 1–13. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brevers, D., He, Q., Xue, G., & Bechara, A. (2017). Neural correlates of the impact of prior outcomes on subsequent monetary decision-making in frequent poker players. Biological Psychology, 124, 30–38. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.01.009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cazzell, M., Li, L., Lin, Z. J., Patel, S. J., & Liu, H. (2012). Comparison of neural correlates of risk decision making between genders: An exploratory fNIRS study of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). NeuroImage, 62(3), 1896–1911. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ciccarelli, M., Griffiths, M. D., Nigro, G., & Cosenza, M. (2017). Decision making, cognitive distortions and emotional distress: A comparison between pathological gamblers and healthy controls. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 54, 204–210. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.08.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cosenza, M., Griffiths, M. D., Nigro, G., & Ciccarelli, M. (2017). Risk-taking, delay discounting, and time perspective in adolescent gamblers: An experimental study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33(2), 383–395. doi:10.1007/s10899-016-9623-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, rationality and the human brain (p. 352). New York: Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Ruiter, M. B., Veltman, D. J., Goudriaan, A. E., Oosterlaan, J., Sjoerds, Z., & van den Brink, W. (2009). Response perseveration and ventral prefrontal sensitivity to reward and punishment in male problem gamblers and smokers. Neuropsychopharmacology : Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 34(4), 1027–1038. doi:10.1038/npp.2008.175;10.1038/npp.2008.175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickerson, M., Hinchy, J., & Fabre, J. (1987). Chasing, arousal and sensation seeking in off-course gamblers. British Journal of Addiction, 82(6), 673–680. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1987.tb01530.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Figner, B., Mackinlay, R. J., Wilkening, F., & Weber, E. U. (2009). Affective and deliberative processes in risky choice: age differences in risk taking in the Columbia Card Task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 709–730. doi:10.1037/a0014983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franken, I. H. A., Georgieva, I., Muris, P., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2006). The rich get richer and the poor get poorer: On risk aversion in behavioral decision-making. Judgment and Decision Making, 1(2), 153–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giorgetta, C., Grecucci, A., Rattin, A., Guerreschi, C., Sanfey, A. G., & Bonini, N. (2014). To play or not to play: A personal dilemma in pathological gambling. Psychiatry Research, 219(3), 562–569. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.06.042.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giorgetta, C., Grecucci, A., Zuanon, S., Perini, L., Balestrieri, M., Bonini, N., et al. (2012). Reduced risk-taking behavior as a trait feature of anxiety. Emotion, 12(6), 1373–1383. doi:10.1037/a0029119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R. L., Hill, C. L., et al. (1989). The Yale-Brown obsessive compulsive scale. I. Development, use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46(11), 1006–1011. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810110048007.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, J. E., Potenza, M. N., Weinstein, A., & Gorelick, D. A. (2010). Introduction to behavioral addictions. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 36(5), 233–241. doi:10.3109/00952990.2010.491884.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Grecucci, A., Giorgetta, C., Rattin, A., Guerreschi, C., Sanfey, A. G., & Bonini, N. (2014). Time devours things: How impulsivity and time affect temporal decisions in pathological gamblers. PLoS ONE, 9(10), e109197. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0109197.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hershey, J. C., & Schoemaker, P. J. H. (1980). Risk taking and problem context in the domain of losses: An expected utility analysis. The Journal of Risk and Insurance, 47(1), 111–132. doi:10.2307/252685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holt, C. A., & Laury, S. K. (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review, 92(5), 1644–1655. doi:10.1257/000282802762024700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, M. K., Hopko, D. R., Bare, R., Lejuez, C. W., & Robinson, E. V. (2005). Construct validity of the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART): Associations with psychopathy and impulsivity. Assessment, 12(4), 416–428. doi:10.1177/1073191105278740.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krmpotich, T., Mikulich-Gilbertson, S., Sakai, J., Thompson, L., Banich, M. T., & Tanabe, J. (2015). Impaired decision-making, higher impulsivity, and drug severity in substance dependence and pathological gambling. Journal of Addiction Medicine, 9(4), 273–280. doi:10.1097/ADM.0000000000000129.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lauriola, M., Panno, A., Levin, I. P., & Lejuez, C. W. (2014). Individual differences in risky decision making : A meta-analysis of sensation seeking and impulsivity with the Balloon Analogue Risk Task. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 36(April 2013), 20–36. doi:10.1002/bdm.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ledgerwood, D. M., Alessi, S. M., Phoenix, N., & Petry, N. M. (2009). Behavioral assessment of impulsivity in pathological gamblers with and without substance use disorder histories versus healthy controls. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 105(1–2), 89–96. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.06.011.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leigh, B. C. (1999). Peril, chance, adventure: concepts of risk, alcohol use and risky behavior in young adults. Addiction, 94(3), 371–383. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.9433717.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lejuez, C. W., Aklin, W., Daughters, S., Zvolensky, M., Kahler, C., & Gwadz, M. (2007). Reliability and validity of the youth version of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART-Y) in the assessment of risk-taking behavior among inner-city adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36(1), 106–111. doi:10.1080/15374410709336573.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lejuez, C. W., Aklin, W. M., Jones, H. A., Richards, J. B., Strong, D. R., Kahler, C. W., et al. (2003). The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) differentiates smokers and nonsmokers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11(1), 26–33. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.11.1.26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., et al. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(2), 75–84. doi:10.1037//1076-898X.8.2.75.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lesieur, H. R. (1979). The compulsive gambler’s spiral of options and involvement. Psychiatry, 42(1), 79–87. doi:10.1521/00332747.1979.11024008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lesieur, H. R., & Blume, S. B. (1987). The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): A new instrument for the identification of Pathological gamblers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144(9), 1184–1188. doi:10.1176/ajp.144.9.1184.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linnet, J., Røjskjær, S., Nygaard, J., & Maher, B. A. (2006). Episodic chasing in pathological gamblers using the Iowa gambling task. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47(1), 43–49. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2006.00491.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lole, L., Gonsalvez, C. J., & Barry, R. J. (2015). Reward and punishment hyposensitivity in problem gamblers: A study of event-related potentials using a principal components analysis. Clinical Neurophysiology, 126(7), 1295–1309. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2014.10.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Losecaat Vermeer, A. B., Boksem, M. A. S., & Sanfey, A. G. (2014). Neural mechanisms underlying context-dependent shifts in risk preferences. NeuroImage, 103, 355–363. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.054.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lynam, D. R., Smith, G. T., Whiteside, S. P., & Cyders, M. A. (2006). The UPPS-P: Assessing five personality pathways to impulsive behavior. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKillop, J., Amlung, M. T., Few, L. R., Ray, L. A., Sweet, L. H., & Munafò, M. R. (2011). Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior: A meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology, 216(3), 305–321. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2229-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, J., & Dickerson, M. (2003). Impaired control over gambling in gaming machine and off-course gamblers. Addiction, 98(1), 53–60. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00232.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pallanti, S., DeCaria, C. M., Grant, J. E., Urpe, M., & Hollander, E. (2005). Reliability and validity of the pathological gambling adaptation of the Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (PG-YBOCS). Journal of Gambling Studies, 21(4), 431–443. doi:10.1007/s10899-005-5557-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(6), 768–774. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768:AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pleskac, T. J., Wallsten, T. S., Wang, P., & Lejuez, C. W. (2008). Development of an automatic response mode to improve the clinical utility of sequential risk-taking tasks. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16(6), 555–564. doi:10.1037/a0014245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reuter, J., Raedler, T., Rose, M., Hand, I., Gläscher, J., & Büchel, C. (2005). Pathological gambling is linked to reduced activation of the mesolimbic reward system. Nature Neuroscience, 8(2), 147–148. doi:10.1038/nn1378.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, T. W., & Clark, L. (2015). Behavioral addictions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 30(February), 66–72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosi, A., Cavallini, E., Gamboz, N., & Russo, R. (2016). On the generality of the effect of experiencing prior gains and losses on the Iowa Gambling Task: A study on young and old adults. Judgment and Decision Making, 11(2), 185–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schellinck, T., & Schrans, T. (1998). The 1998 Nova Scotia video lottery survey. Canada: Halifax.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1984). Selection by consequences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7(4), 477–481. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0002673X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E. M., & MacGregor, D. G. (2007). The affect heuristic. European Journal of Operational Research, 177(3), 1333–1352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stinchfield, R. (2002). Reliability, validity, and classification accuracy of the South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS). Addictive Behaviors, 27(1), 1–19. doi:10.1016/S0306-4603(00)00158-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tanabe, J., Thompson, L., Claus, E., Dalwani, M., Hutchison, K., & Banich, M. T. (2007). Prefrontal cortex activity is reduced in gambling and nongambling substance users during decision-making. Human Brain Mapping, 28(12), 1276–1286. doi:10.1002/hbm.20344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. H., & Johnson, E. J. (1990). Gambling with the house money and trying to break even: The effects of prior outcomes on risky choice. Management Science, 36(6), 643–660. doi:10.1287/mnsc.36.6.643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Review, 5(5), 551–553. doi:10.1037/h0067373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toce-Gerstein, M., Gerstein, D. R., & Volberg, R. A. (2003). A hierarchy of gambling disorders in the community. Addiction, 98(12), 1661–1672. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2003.00545.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vermeer, A. B. L., & Sanfey, A. G. (2015). The effect of positive and negative feedback on risk-taking across different contexts. PLoS ONE, 10(9), e0139010. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0139010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, M. B., & Dickerson, M. G. (1996). The prevalence of problem and pathological gambling: A critical analysis. Journal of Gambling Studies, 12(2), 233–249. doi:10.1007/BF01539176.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, M., & Zuchel, H. (2005). How do prior outcomes affect risk attitude? Comparing escalation of commitment and the house-money effect. Decision Analysis, 2(1), 30–43. doi:10.1287/deca.1050.0034.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, T. L., Lejuez, C. W., & de Wit, H. (2008). Test-retest characteristics of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16(6), 565–570. doi:10.1037/a0014083.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(4), 669–689. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00064-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization. (1993). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva: World Health Organization.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xue, G., Lu, Z., Levin, I. P., & Bechara, A. (2010). The impact of prior risk experiences on subsequent risky decision-making: The role of the insula. NeuroImage, 50(2), 709–716. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.097.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Xue, G., Lu, Z., Levin, I. P., & Bechara, A. (2011). An fMRI study of risk-taking following wins and losses: Implications for the gambler’s fallacy. Human Brain Mapping, 32(2), 271–281. doi:10.1002/hbm.21015.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This study was funded by the Autonomous Province of Trento (PAT) (“Pre.GIO.—Monitoraggio e prevenzione del gioco d’azzardo patologico in Trentino”, Atto esecutivo 2013, Area di ricerca 1—Rapporti sulla sicurezza in Trentino—Linea di attività 2). This experiment (Nicolao Bonini, P.I.) was part of that more extensive research project.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Nicolao Bonini or Lucia Savadori.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Nicolao Bonini declares that he has no conflict of interest. Alessandro Grecucci declares that he has no conflict of interest. Manuel Nicolè declares that he has no conflict of interest. Lucia Savadori declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bonini, N., Grecucci, A., Nicolè, M. et al. Reduced Risk-Taking After Prior Losses in Pathological Gamblers Under Treatment and Healthy Control Group but not in Problem Gamblers. J Gambl Stud 34, 429–447 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-017-9709-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-017-9709-z

Keywords

Navigation