Journal of Gambling Studies

, Volume 24, Issue 3, pp 275–293 | Cite as

The Desire to Gamble: The Influence of Outcomes on the Priming Effects of a Gambling Episode

  • Matthew M. Young
  • Michael J. A. Wohl
  • Kimberly Matheson
  • Steve Baumann
  • Hymie Anisman
Original Paper

Abstract

The influence of gambling outcomes on the efficacy of a short gambling episode to prime motivation to continue gambling was determined in two experiments in which desire to gamble was evaluated while participants played a slot machine located in a virtual reality casino. In experiment 1, 38 high-risk [>3 Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI)] [Ferris and Wynne (The Canadian problem gambling index: final report, 2001)] and 36 non-problem gamblers (0 PGSI) either won or lost a modest amount. Among high-risk gamblers, winning resulted in a greater increase in the desire to continue gambling than did losing. In experiment 2, 39 high-risk, 33 low-risk (0 < PGSI < 3), and 31 non-problem gamblers experienced either a single large win or a series of small wins (equivalent monetary gain). Participants were permitted to continue playing as long as they wanted (all subsequent spins being losses) thus permitting evaluation of persistence (resistance to extinction). Throughout, desire to gamble was assessed using a single item measure. High-risk gamblers who experienced a large win reported significantly greater desire to gamble upon voluntary cessation than those who experienced a series of small wins. It seems that the priming effects of a short gambling episode are contingent on the pattern of outcomes experienced by the gambler. The data were related to motivational factors associated with gambling, gambling persistence, and chasing losses.

Keywords

Priming Craving Gambling Virtual reality Slot machines Pathology 

References

  1. Alcañiz, M., Perpiña, C., Baños, R., Lozano, J. A., Montesa, J., Botella, C., et al. (2000). A new realistic 3D body representation in virtual environments for the treatment of disturbed body image in eating disorders. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 3, 433–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. APA. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  3. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman & Co.Google Scholar
  4. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical consideration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baumann, S. B., Fetzick, S., Stangl, G., Brauch, K., Kenney, J., Yothers, T., et al. (2006). A flexible virtual reality cue-exposure platform for drug abuse studies. Paper presented at the 11th Annual Cyber Therapy Conference, Gatineau, Québec.Google Scholar
  6. Baumann, S. B., Neff, C., Fetzick, S., Stangl, G., Basler, L., Vereneck, R., et al. (2003). A virtual reality system for neurobehavioral and functional MRI studies. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 6, 259–266.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Baumann, S. B., & Sayette, M. A. (2006). Smoking cues in a virtual world provoke craving in cigarette smokers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20, 484–489.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brown, S. L., Rodda, S., & Phillips, J. G. (2004). Differences between problem and nonproblem gamblers in subjective arousal and affective valence amongst electronic gaming machine players. Addictive Behaviors, 29, 1863–1867.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Castellani, B., & Rugle, L. (1995). A comparison of pathological gamblers to alcoholics and cocaine misusers on impulsivity, sensation seeking, and craving. International Journal of the Addictions, 30, 275–289.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Chutuape, M. A., Mitchell, S., & DeWit, H. (1994). Ethanol preloads increase ethonal preference in social drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 2, 29–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Clarke, D., Tse, S., Abbott, M. W., Townsend, S., Kingi, P., & Manaia, W. (2007). Reasons for starting and continuing gambling in a mixed ethnic community sample of pathological and non-problem gamblers. International Gambling Studies, 7, 299–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cote, D., Caron, A., Aubert, J., Desrochers, V., & Ladouceur, R. (2003). Near wins prolong gambling on a video lottery terminal. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19, 433–438.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Coventry, K. R., & Constable, B. (1999). Physiological arousal and sensation-seeking in female fruit machine gamblers. Addiction, 94, 425–430.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Coventry, K. R., & Hudson, J. (2001). Gender differences, physiological arousal and the role of winning in fruit machine gamblers. Addiction, 96, 871–879.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Crockford, D. N., & el-Guebaly, N. (1998). Naltrexone in the treatment of pathological gambling and alcohol dependence. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 43, 86.Google Scholar
  16. Crockford, D. N., Goodyear, J. E., Edwards, J., Quickfall, J., & el-Guebaly, N. (2005). Cue-induced brain activity in pathological gamblers. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 787–795.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Custer, R., & Milt, H. (1985). When luck runs out: Help for compulsive gamblers and their families. New York: Facts on File Publications.Google Scholar
  18. Darke, P. R., & Freedman, J. L. (1997). The belief in good luck scale. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 486–511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. DeWit, H. (1996). Priming effects with drugs and other reinforcers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 4, 5–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Drummond, D. C. (2001). Theories of drug craving, ancient and modern. Addiction, 96, 33–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Drummond, D. C., Litten, R. Z., Lowman, C., & Hunt, W. A. (2000). Craving research: future directions. Addiction, 95, S247–S255.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ferris, J., & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian problem gambling index: Final report. Ottawa: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse.Google Scholar
  23. Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
  24. Griffiths, M. D. (1999). The psychology of the near miss (revisited): A comment on Delfabbro and Winefield. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 441–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hills, A. M., Hill, S., Mamone, N., & Dickerson, M. (2001). Induced mood and persistence at gaming. Addiction, 96, 1629–1638.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Holub, A., Hodgins, D. C., & Peden, N. E. (2005). Development of the temptations for gambling questionnaire: A measure of temptation in recently quit gamblers. Addiction Research and Theory, 13, 179–191.Google Scholar
  27. Juliano, L. M., & Brandon, T. H. (1998). Reactivity to instructed smoking availability and environmental cues: Evidence with urge and reaction time. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 6, 45–53.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kassinove, J. I., & Schare, M. L. (2001). Effects of the “Near Miss” and the “Big Win” on persistence at slot machine gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15, 155–158.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kelly, O., Matheson, K., Ravindran, A. V., Merali, Z., & Anisman, H. (2007). Coping profiles among dysthymic patients before and after pharmacotherapy. Anxiety & Depression, 24, 233–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kozlowski, L. T., Pillitteri, J. L., Sweeney, C. T., Whitfield, K. E., & Graham, J. W. (1996). Asking questions about urges or cravings for cigarettes. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 10, 248–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Ladouceur, R., Mayrand, M., & Tourigny, Y. (1987). Risk-taking behavior in gamblers and nongamblers during prolongud exposure. Journal of Gambling Behavior, 3, 115–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ladouceur, R., & Sevigny, S. (2002). Symbols presentation modality as determinant of gambling behavior. The Journal of Psychology, 136, 443–448.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Ladouceur, R., & Sevigny, S. (2005). Structural characteristics of video lottery terminals: Effects of of a stopping device on illusion of control and gambling persistence. Journal of Gambling Studies, 21, 117–131.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Ladouceur, R., Sevigny, S., Blaszczynski, A., O’Connor, K., & Lavoie, M. E. (2003). Video lottery: Winning expectancies and arousal. Addiction, 98, 733–738.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lesieur, H. R. (1984). The Chase: Career of the compulsive gambler. Cambridge, MA: Schenkman.Google Scholar
  36. Litt, M. D., & Cooney, N. L. (1999). Inducing craving for alcohol in the laboratory. Alcohol Research and Health, 33, 174–178.Google Scholar
  37. Loba, P., Stewart, S. H., Klein, R. M., & Blackburn, J. R. (2001). Manipulations of the features of standard video lottery terminal (VLT) games: effects in pathological and non-pathological gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 17, 297–320.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ludwig, A. M., & Wikler, A. (1974). “Craving” and relapse to drink. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 35, 108–130.Google Scholar
  39. Marlatt, G. A., & Gordon, J. R. (1985). Relapse prevention. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  40. Marshall, K., & Wynne, H. (2003). Fighting the odds. Perspectives on Labour and Income, 4, 5–13.Google Scholar
  41. Neighbors, C., Lostutter, T., Cronce, J., & Larimer, M. (2002). Exploring college student gambling motivation. Journal of Gambling Studies, 18, 361–370.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. O’Connor, J., & Dickerson, M. (2003). Definition and measurement of chasing in off course betting and gaming machine play. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19, 359–385.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Orford, J. (2001). Excessive appetites: A psychological view of addictions (2nd ed.). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  44. Potenza, M. N., Steinberg, M. A., Skudlarski, P., Fulbright, R. K., Lacadie, C. M., Wilber, M. K., et al. (2003). Gambling urges in pathological gambling: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 828–836.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 717–731.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Raylu, N., & Oei, T. P. S. (2004). The gambling urge scale: Development, confirmatory factor validation, and psychometric properties. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 18, 100–105.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Rush, R., Moxam, R. S., & Urbanoski, K. A. (2002). Characteristics of people seeking help from specialized programs for the treatment of problem gambling in Ontario. eGambling, 6, 1–23.Google Scholar
  48. Sayette, M. A., Shiffman, S., Tiffany, S. T., Niaura, R. S., Martin, C. S., & Schadel, W. G. (2000). The measurement of drug craving. Addiction, 95, S189–S210.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Schellinck, T., & Schrans, T. (1998). Nova Scotia video lottery players’ survey. Report for the Nova Scotia Department of Health, Halifax, Nova Scotia.Google Scholar
  50. Shiffman, S., Paty, J. A., Gnys, M., Kassel, J. A., & Hickcox, M. (1996). First Lapses to smoking: Within-subjects analysis of real-time reports. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 366–379.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behaviour. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  52. Smith, G. J., & Wynne, H. (2004). VLT gambling in Alberta: A preliminary analysis: Alberta Gambling Research Institute.Google Scholar
  53. Steenbergh, T. A., Meyers, A. W., May, R. K., & Whelan, J. P. (2002). Development and validation of the gamblers’ beliefs questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16, 143–149.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Strickland, L. H., & Grote, F. W. (1967). Temporal presentation of winning symbols and slot machine playing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 10–13.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Tavares, H., Zilberman, M. L., Hodgins, D. C., & el-Guebaly, N. (2005). Comparison of craving between pathological gamblers and alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29, 1427–1431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Tiffany, S. T., Carter, B. L., & Singleton, E. G. (2000). Challenges in the manipulation, assessment and interpretation of craving relevant variables. Addiction, 95, S177–S187.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Toneatto, T., Blitz-Miller, T., Calderwook, K., Dragonetti, R., & Tsanos, A. (1997). Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 13, 253–266.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Verheul, R., Van den Brink, W., & Geerlings, P. (1999). A three-pathway psychobiological model of craving for alcohol. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 34, 197–222.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Weatherly, J. N., Sauter, J. M., & King, B. M. (2004). The “big win” and resistance to extinction when gambling. Journal of Psychology, 138, 495–504.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Wiebe, J., Mun, P., & Kauffman, N. (2006). Gambling and problem gambling in Ontario. Toronto, Ontario: Responsible Gambling Council.Google Scholar
  61. Wiebe, J., Single, E., & Falkowski-Ham, A. (2001). Measuring gambling and problem gambling in Ontario. Ontario: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse, Responsible Gambling Council.Google Scholar
  62. Wiederhold, B. K., & Wiederhold, M. D. (2000). Lessons learned from 600 virtual reality sessions. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 3, 393–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Wise, R. A. (2004). Drive, incentive, and reinforcement: The antecedents and consequences of motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 50, 159–195.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Wohl, M. J. A. (in press). Belief in a lucky self: The role of personal luck in the facilitation and maintenance of gambling behavior. Psychologie Francaise.Google Scholar
  65. Wohl, M. J. A., Anisman, H., Matheson, K., & Young, M. M. (2006). Personal luck, outcome expectancies, and subjective appraisals as predictors of health and readiness to seek treatment among young adults. Ottawa: Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre.Google Scholar
  66. Wohl, M. J. A., & Enzle, M. E. (2002). The deployment of personal luck: Illusory control in games of pure chance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1388–1397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Wohl, M. J. A., & Enzle, M. E. (2003). The effects of near wins and losses on self perceived personal luck and subsequent gambling behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 184–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Wohl, M. J. A., Young, M. M., & Hart, K. E. (2005). Untreated young gamblers with game-specific problems: Self-concept involving luck, gambling ecology and delay in seeking professional treatment. Addiction Research and Theory, 13, 445–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Wohl, M. J. A., Young, M. M., & Hart, K. E. (2007). Self-perceptions of dispositional luck: Relationship to DSM gambling symptoms, subjective enjoyment of gambling and treatment readiness. Substance Use and Misuse, 42, 43–63.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Zack, M., & Poulos, C. X. (2004). Amphetamine primes motivation to gamble and gambling related semantic networks in problem gamblers. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29, 195–207.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Zack, M., & Poulos, C. X. (2007). A D2 antagonist enhances the rewarding and priming effects of a gambling episode in pathological gamblers. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32, 1678–1686.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Matthew M. Young
    • 1
  • Michael J. A. Wohl
    • 1
  • Kimberly Matheson
    • 1
  • Steve Baumann
    • 2
  • Hymie Anisman
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyCarleton UniversityOttawaCanada
  2. 2.Psychology Software ToolsPittsburghUSA

Personalised recommendations