Advertisement

The Relationship Between Symptoms and Abilities in Autism

  • Murray J. DyckEmail author
  • Jan P. Piek
  • David A. Hay
  • Joachim F. Hallmayer
Original Article

Abstract

We assessed if autism symptoms relate to ability deficits and achievement discrepancies in 29 children aged 4–13 years with a diagnosis of Autistic Disorder. Symptoms, intelligence, language, motor coordination and social cognition were assessed. Children with autism underachieve on all ability measures. There were significant achievement discrepancies between Performance IQ and theory of mind, fine and gross motor coordination. The most common discrepancy was between Performance IQ scores and motor coordination scores, which was observed in 86% of children. Early developmental abnormalities related to most abilities, social interaction symptoms related to motor coordination and receptive language, and symptoms related to discrepancies between PIQ and social cognitive abilities.

Keywords

Autism Intelligence Empathy Motor coordination Language 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by project grant #141107 from the National Health and Medical Research Council. We wish to thank all of the children and parents who helped us with this study.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  2. Baranek, G. (1999). Autism during infancy: A retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor and social behaviors at 9–12 months of age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 213–224.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition, 21, 37–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test Revised Version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger Syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Belmonte, M., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2003). Functional anatomy of impaired selective attention and compensatory processing in autism. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 651–664.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Berument, S., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. (1999). Autism screening questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 444–451.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dyck, M., Farrugia, C., Shochet, I., & Holmes-Brown, M. (2004a). Emotion recognition/understanding ability in hearing or vision-impaired children: Do sounds, sights, or words make the difference? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 789–800.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dyck, M., Ferguson, K., & Shochet, I. (2001). Do autism spectrum disorders differ from each other and from non-spectrum disorders on emotion recognition tests? European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 10, 105–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dyck, M., Hay, D., Anderson, M., Smith, L., Piek, J., & Hallmayer, J. (2004b). Is the discrepancy criterion for defining developmental disorders valid? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 979–995.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dyck, M., Piek, J., Hay, D., Smith, L., & Hallmayer, J. (2006). Are abilities abnormally interdependent in children with autism? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35, 20–33.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Ehlers, S., Nyden, A., Gillberg, C., Sandberg, A., Dahlgren, S., Hjelmquist, E., et al. (1997). Asperger syndrome, autism and attention disorders: A comparative study of the cognitive profiles of 120 children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 207–217.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Frith, U. (1996). Social communication and its disorder in autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 10, 48–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gepner, B., & Mestre, D. (2002). Brief report: Postural reactivity to fast visual motion differentiates autistic from children with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 231–238.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Ghaziuddin, M., & Butler, E. (1998). Clumsiness in autism and Asperger syndrome: A further report. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 42, 43–48.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Goldstein, G., Beers, S., Siegel, D., & Minshew, N. (2001). A comparison of the WAIS-R profiles in adults with high-functioning autism or differing subtypes of learning disability. Applied Neuropsychology, 8, 148–154.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Happe, F. (1994a). Wechsler IQ profile and theory of mind in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1461–1471.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Happe, F. (1994b). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Harris, P., Johnson, C., Hutton, D., Andrews, G., & Cooke, T. (1989). Young children’s theory of mind and emotion. Cognition & Emotion, 3, 379–400.Google Scholar
  19. Heinze, H., Mangun, G., Burchert, W., Hinrichs, H., Scholz, M., Munte, T., et al. (1994). Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans. Nature, 372, 543–546.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning adults with autism with and without early language delays: Implications for the differentiation between autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 3–13.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Matsumoto, D., & Ekman, P. (1995). Japanese And Caucasian Facial Expressions Of Emotion (JACFEE) And Neutral Faces (JACNeuF). San Francisco: San Francisco State University.Google Scholar
  23. McCarron, L. (1997). McCarron assessment of neuromuscular development: Fine and gross motor abilities (revised). Texas: McCarron-Dial Systems, Inc.Google Scholar
  24. Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 255–263.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Molloy, C., Dietrich, K., & Bhattacharya, A. (2003). Postural stability in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 643–652.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mottron, L., Burack, J., Iarocci, G., Belleville, S., & Enns, J. (2003). Locally oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high-functioning autism: Evidence from multiple paradigms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 904–913.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mottron, L., Peretz, I., & Menard, E. (2000). Local and global processing of music in high-functioning persons with autism: Beyond central coherence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 1057–1063.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A., & Leekam, S. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child’s theory of mind: Knowledge belief and communication. Child Development, 60, 689–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that...” Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5-to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437–471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 765–775.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Rinehart, N., Bradshaw, J., Moss, S., Brereton, A., & Tonge, B. (2000). Atypical interference of local detail on global processing in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s Disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 769–778.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2002). Shape constancy in autism: The role of prior knowledge and perspective cues. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 647–653.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Berument, S., Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (2001). The social communication questionnaire: Current version. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  34. Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (1995). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals: Technical manual (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  35. Volkmar, F., Lord, C., Bailey, A., Schultz, R., & Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 135–170.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children—Third edition: Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  37. Wilson, P., & McKenzie, B. (1998). Information processing deficits associated with Developmental Coordination Disorder: A meta analysis of research findings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 829–840.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Murray J. Dyck
    • 1
    Email author
  • Jan P. Piek
    • 2
  • David A. Hay
    • 2
  • Joachim F. Hallmayer
    • 3
  1. 1.School of PsychologyGriffith UniversityQLDAustralia
  2. 2.School of PsychologyCurtin University of TechnologyBentleyAustralia
  3. 3.Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral SciencesStanford UniversityStanfordUSA

Personalised recommendations