Attribution of Emotions to Story Characters by Severely and Profoundly Deaf Children

  • Colin Gray
  • Judith Hosie
  • Phil Russell
  • Christine Scott
  • Norma Hunter
Article

Abstract

Simple stories were presented to severely or profoundly prelingually deafened children and hearing controls in two age groups. Participants were asked to choose the appropriate emotion for the central characters. While at either age level, the hearing children's success rate was significantly higher than that of the deaf children, the older deaf children showed a marked improvement compared with the younger deaf children. In each age group, the profiles of the average scores of the deaf children over the six emotions studied were similar to those of their hearing peers, suggesting that the deaf children found the same emotions easier (or more difficult) to assign to the story characters. It would appear that deaf and hearing children code their emotional experiences using the same categories, which are progressively refined with age. The developmental delay in deaf children's emotional understanding is interpreted in terms of the social experience of the deaf child of hearing parents, which affords limited opportunity to discuss emotional causality.

Key Words

deafness story understanding facial expressions emotion 

REFERENCES

  1. British Association of Teachers of the Deaf (1981). Audiological definitions and forms for recording audiometric information. J. Brit. Assoc. Teach. Deaf 5: 83–87.Google Scholar
  2. Brody, L., and Harrison, R. (1987). Developmental changes in children's abilities to match and label emotionally laden situations. Motiv. Emotion 11: 347–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bullock, M., and Russell, J. A. (1984). Preschool children's interpretation of facial expressions of emotion. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 7: 193–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Carlson, C., Felleman, E., and Masters, J. (1983). Influence of children's emotional states on the recognition of emotion in peers and social motives to change another's emotional state. Motiv. Emotion 7: 61–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Clarke School for the Deaf (1953). 86th Annual Report, Northampton, MA.Google Scholar
  6. DeConti, K. A., and Dickerson, D. J. (1994). Preschool childrens understanding of the situational determinants of others emotions. Cogn. Emotion 8: 453–472.Google Scholar
  7. Denham, S. (1986a). Affective understanding in young preschoolers and reactions to peers’ emotions. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  8. Denham, S. A. (1986b). Social cognition, prosocial behaviour, and emotion in preschoolers: contextual validation. Child Dev. 57: 194–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Denham, S. A., and Couchoud, E. A. (1990). Young preschoolers’ understanding of emotion. Child Study J. 20: 171–192.Google Scholar
  10. Dorr, A. (1985). Contexts for experience with emotion, with special attention to television. In Lewis, M. and Saarni, C. (eds.), The Socialization of Emotion, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 55–85.Google Scholar
  11. Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., and Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Dev. Psychol. 23: 132–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dunn, J., Brown, J., and Beardsall, L. (1991). Family talk about feeling states and children's later understanding of others’ emotions. Dev. Psychol. 27: 448–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dunn, J., and Brown, J. R. (1993). Early conversations about causality: content, pragmatics and developmental change. Brit. J. Dev. Psychol.Google Scholar
  14. Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Nyman, M., and Michealieu, Q. (1991). Young children's appraisal of others’ spontaneous emotional reactions. Dev. Psychol. 27: 858–866.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fehr, B. J., and Exline, R. V. (1987). Social visual interaction: A conceptual and literature review. In Siegman, A. W., and Feldstein, S. (eds.), Nonverbal Behavior and Communication, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp. 225–326.Google Scholar
  16. Gray, C., Banks, J., Fyfe, R., and Morris, A. (1992). The use of verbatim and schematic strategies in the recall of written stories by deaf and hearing children. Brit. J. Educ. Psychol. 62: 88–105.Google Scholar
  17. Gray, C.D., Hosie, J.A., Russell, P., and Ormel, E. (2001). Emotional development in deaf children: Understanding facial expressions, display rules and theory of mind. In Marschark, M., Clark, D., and Karchmer, M. (eds.), Context, Cognition and Deafness, Gallaudet University Press, Washington, pp. 135–160.Google Scholar
  18. Gregory, S. (1976). The Deaf Child and His Family, Halsted Press, New York.Google Scholar
  19. Hadwin, J., and Perner, J. (1991). Pleased and surprised: Children's cognitive theory of emotion. Brit. J. Dev. Psychol. 9: 215–234.Google Scholar
  20. Harris, D. B. (1963). Children's Drawings as Measures of Intellectual Maturity, Harcourt, Brace and World, New York.Google Scholar
  21. Harris, P. L. (1985). What children know about the situations that provoke emotion. In Lewis, M., and Saarni, C. (eds.), The Socialization of Emotion, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 161–185.Google Scholar
  22. Higgenbotham, D. J., and Baker, B. M. (1981). Social participation and cognitive play differences in hearing-impaired and normally hearing preschoolers. Volta Rev. 83: 135–149.Google Scholar
  23. Hosie, J. A., Gray, C. D., Russell, P. A., Scott, C., and Hunter, N. (1998). The matching of facial expressions by deaf and hearing children and their production and comprehension of emotion labels. Motiv. Emotion 22: 293–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hosie, J. A., Russell, P. A., Gray, C. D., Scott, C., Hunter, N., Banks, J. S., and Macaulay, M. C. (2000). Knowledge of display rules in prelingually deaf and hearing children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 41: 389–398.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Ireson, J., and Shields, M. (1982). The development of the young child's representation of emotion (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 263 983). London, England: University of London Institute of Education.Google Scholar
  26. Lederberg, A. R., and Mobley, C. E. (1990). The effect of hearing impairment on the quality of attachment and mother-toddler interaction. Child Dev. 61: 1596–1604.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Markham, R., and Adams, K. (1992). The effect of type of task on children's identification of facial expressions. J. Nonverbal Behav. 16: 21–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Michalson, L., and Lewis, M. (1985). What do children know about emotions and when do they know it? In Lewis, M., and Saarni, C. (eds.), The Socialization of Emotions, Plenum, New York, pp. 117–139.Google Scholar
  29. Odom, P. B., Blanton, R. L., and Laukhuf, C. (1973). Facial expressions and interpretation of emotion-arousing situations in deaf and hearing children. J. Abnormal Child Psychol. 1: 139–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Peterson, C. C., and Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversation and theory of mind. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 36: 459–474.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Peterson, C. C., and Siegal, M. (1997). Psychological, biological and physical thinking in normal, autistic and deaf children. In Wellman, H. M., and Inagaki, K. (eds.), The Emergence of Core Domains of Thought, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp. 55–70.Google Scholar
  32. Reichenbach, L., and Masters, J. C. (1983). Childrens use of expressive and contextual cues in judgements of emotion. Child Dev. 54: 993–1004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Remmel, E., Bettger, J. G., and Weinberg, A. M. (2001). Theory of mind development in deaf children. In Clark, M. D., Marschark, M., and Karchmer, M. (eds.), Context, Cognition and Deafness, Gallaudet University Press, Washington, DC, pp. 113–134.Google Scholar
  34. Rieffe, C., and Meerum Terwogt, M. (2000). Deaf children's understanding of emotions: Desires take preference. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 41: 601–608.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39: 1161–1178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Russell, J. A., and Bullock, M. (1985). Multidimensional scaling of emotional facial expressions: Similarity from preschoolers to adults. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 48: 1290–1298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Russell, J. A., and Bullock, M. (1986). On the dimensions preschoolers use to interpret facial expressions of emotion. Dev. Psychol. 22: 97–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Russell, J. A., Suzuki, N., and Ishida, N. (1993). Canadian, Greek and Japanese freely produced emotion labels for facial expressions. Motiv. Emotion 17: 337–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Spelke, E. S., and Cortelyou, A. (1981). Perceptual aspects of social knowing: Looking and listening in infancy. In Lamb, M. E., and Sherrod, L. R. (eds.), Infant Social Cognition: Empirical and Theoretical Considerations, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 61–84.Google Scholar
  40. Stein, N. (1979). How children understand stories: A developmental analysis. In Katz, L. G. (ed.), Current Topics in Early Childhood, Ablex, Norwood, N. J., Vol. 2, pp. 261–290.Google Scholar
  41. Stein, N., and Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In Freedle, R. (ed.), Advances in Discourse Processes: New directions in Discourse Processing, Ablex, Norwood, N. J., Vol. 2, pp. 261–190.Google Scholar
  42. Vandell, D., and George, L. (1981). Social interaction in hearing and deaf preschoolers: Successes and failures in initiation. Child Dev. 52: 627–635.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Walden, T. A. (1991). Infant social referencing. In Garber, J., and Dodge, K. A. (eds.), The Development of Emotion Regulation and Dysregulation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 69–88.Google Scholar
  44. Wedell-Monnig, J., and Lumley, J. M. (1980). Child deafness and mother-child interaction. Child Dev. 51: 766–774.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Wellman, H. M., Harris, P. L., Banerjee, M., and Sinclair, A. (1995). Early understanding of emotion: Evidence from natural language. Cognition Emotion 2(3): 117–149.Google Scholar
  46. Wertheimer, M. (1961). Psychomotor coordination of auditory and visual space at birth. Science 134: 1692.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Colin Gray
    • 1
    • 2
  • Judith Hosie
    • 1
  • Phil Russell
    • 1
  • Christine Scott
    • 1
  • Norma Hunter
    • 1
  1. 1.School of PsychologyUniversity of AberdeenScotlandU.K.
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Aberdeen, King’s College, Old AberdeenScotlandU.K.

Personalised recommendations