Advertisement

Journal of Behavioral Medicine

, Volume 30, Issue 3, pp 221–231 | Cite as

Attentional Strategy Moderates Effects of Pain Catastrophizing on Symptom-Specific Physiological Responses in Chronic Low Back Pain Patients

  • Phillip J. Quartana
  • John W. Burns
  • Kenneth R. Lofland
Article

Abstract

In the present study, we examined whether experimentally-manipulated attentional strategies moderated relations between pain catastrophizing and symptom-specific physiological responses to a cold-pressor task among sixty-eight chronic low back patients. Patients completed measures of pain catastrophizing and depression, and were randomly assigned to sensory focus, distraction or suppression conditions during a cold pressor. Lumbar paraspinal and trapezius EMG, and cardiovascular responses to the cold pressor were assessed. Attentional strategies moderated the relation between pain catastrophizing and lumbar paraspinal muscle, but not trapezius muscle or cardiovascular responses. Only for participants in the suppression condition was catastrophizing related significantly to lumbar paraspinal muscle responses. Depressed affect did not account for this relation. These findings indicate that ‘symptom-specific’ responses among pain catastrophizers with chronic low back depend on how they attend to pain-related information. Specifically, it appears that efforts to suppress awareness of pain exaggerate muscular responses near the site of injury.

Keywords

Chronic pain Symptom-specific reactivity Pain catastrophizing Attention Cognition Suppression 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The authors gratefully acknowledge the staff at the Pain and Rehabilitation Clinic of Chicago, without which this project would not have been possible.

This research was partly supported by a Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (1 F31 NS051200-01A1) from the National Institutes of Neurological Disorders and Stroke awarded to Phillip J. Quartana, and Grant NS37164 from the National Institutes of Neurological Disorders and Stroke awarded to John W. Burns.

References

  1. Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  2. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, B. F. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  3. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bonica, J. J. (1990). Definitions and taxonomy of pain. In Bonica JJ (Ed.), The management of pain. Philadelphia PA: Lea and Febiger.Google Scholar
  5. Burns, J. W. (2006a). The role of attentional strategies in moderating the links between acute pain-induction and subsequent psychological stress: evidence for symptom-specific reactivity among chronic pain patients versus healthy controls. Emotion, 6, 180–192.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Burns, J. W. (2006b). Arousal of negative emotions and symptom-specific reactivity in chronic low back pain patients. Emotion, 6, 309–319.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Burns, J. W., Wiegner, S., Derleth, M., Kiselica, K., & Pawl, R. (1997). Linking symptom-specific physiological reactivity to pain severity in chronic low back pain patients: a test of medication and moderation models. Health Psychology, 4, 319–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chaves, J. F., & Brown, J. M. (1987). Spontaneous cognitive strategies for the control of clinical pain and stress. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10, 263–276.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cioffi, D., & Hollway, J. (1993). Delayed costs of suppressed pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 274–282.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  11. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analyses for the behavioral sciences (2nd Ed.). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  12. Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., Baeyens, F., & Eelen, P. (1998). When somatic information threatens, catastrophic thinking enhances attentional interference. Pain, 75, 187–198.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., Van der Broeck, A., Goubert, L., & Van Houndenhove, B. (2004). Hypervigilance to pain in fibromyalgia: the mediating role of pain intensity and catastrophic thinking about pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, 20, 98–102.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dar, R., & Leventhal, H. (1993). Schematic processes in pain perception. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 17, 341–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dolce, J. J., & Raczynski, J. M. (1985). Neuromuscular activity and electromyography in painful backs: psychological and biomedical models in assessment and treatment. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 502–520.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Eccleston C (1995). Chronic pain and distraction: an experimental investigation into the role of sustained and shifting attention in the processing of chronic persistent pain. Behavior Research and Therapy, 33, 391–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Eccleston, C., & Crombez G (1999). Pain demands attention: a cognitive-affective model on the interruptive function of pain. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 256–366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Edens, J. L., & Gil, K. M. (1995). Experimental induction of pain: Utility in the study of clinical pain. Behavior Therapy, 26, 197–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Flor, H., Birbaumer, N., Schugens, M. M., & Lutzenberger, W. (1992) Symptom-specific psychophysiological responses in chronic pain patients. Psychophysiology, 29, 452–460.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Flor, H., Knost, B., & Birbaumer, N. (2002). The role of operant conditioning in chronic pain: an experimental investigation. Pain, 95, 111–118.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Flor, H., & Turk, D. C. (1989). Psychophysiology of chronic pain: do chronic pain patients exhibit symptom-specific psychophysiological responses? Psychological Bulletin, 105, 215–259.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Flor, H., Turk, D. C., & Birbaumer, N. (1985). Assessment of stress-related psychophysiological reaction in chronic pain patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 354–364.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. France, C. R., France, J. L., al’Absi M., Ring, C., & McIntyre, D. (2002). Catastrophizing is related to pain ratings, but not nociceptive flexion reflex threshold. Pain, 99, 459–463.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fridlund, A. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human electromyographic research. Psychophysiology, 23, 567–589.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Goubert, L., Crombex, G., Eccleston, C., & Devulder, J. (2004) Distraction from chronic pain during a pain-inducing activity is associated with greater post-activity pain. Pain, 110, 220–227.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Geisser, M. E., Ranavaya, M., Haig, A. J., Roth, R. S., Zucker, R., Ambroz, C., Caruso, M. (2005). A meta-analytic review of surface electromyography among persons with low back pain and normal, healthy controls. Journal of Pain, 6, 711–726.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Keppel, G., & Zedeck, S. (1989). Data analysis for research designs: analysis of variance and multiple regression/correlation approaches. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
  28. Koster, E. H. W., Rassin, E., Crombez, G., & Naring, G. W. B. (2003). The paradoxical effects of suppressing anxious thoughts during imminent threat. Behavior Research and Therapy, 41, 1113–1120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Leventhal, H., Bron, D., Shacham, S., Engquist, G. (1979). Effects of prepatory information about sensations, threat of pain, and attention on cold pressor distress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 688–714.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lundberg, U., Dohns, I. E., Melin, B., Sandsjo, L., Palmeund, G., Kadefors, R., Ekstron, M., & Parr D (1999). Psychophysiological stress responses, muscle tension, and neck and shoulder pain among supermarket cashiers. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 4, 245–255.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. McCracken, L. M., & Eccleston, C. (2005). A prospective study of acceptance of pain and patient functioning with chronic pain. Pain, 118, 164–169.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Michael, E., & Burns, J. W. (2004). Catastrophizing and pain tolerance among chronic pain patients: moderating effects of sensory and affect focus. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 27, 185–194.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Rosentiel, A. K., & Keefe, F. J. (1983). The use of coping strategies in chronic low back pain patients: Relationship to patient characteristics and current adjustment. Pain, 17, 33–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing : II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychology Review, 84, 127–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Sjegaard, G., Lundberg, U., & Kudefors, R. (2000) The role of muscle activity and mental load in the development of pain and degenerative processes of the muscle cell level during computer work. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 83, 99–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Spanos, N. P., Radtke-Brodorik, H. L., Freguson, J. D., & Jones (1979). The effects of hypnotic susceptibility, suggestions for analgesia, and utilization of cognitive strategies on the reduction of pain. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 282–292.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Spence, S. H., Sharpe, L., Newton-John, T., & Champion, D. (2001). An investigation of symptom-specific muscle hyperreactivity in upper expremity cumulative trauma disorder. Clinical Journal of Pain, 17, 119–128.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Sullivan, M. J. L., Bishop, S. R., & Pivik, J. (1995). The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: development and validation. Psychological Assessment, 7, 524–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Sullivan, M. J. L., & D’eon J. (1990). Relation between catastrophizing and depression in chronic pain patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 260–63.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Sullivan, M. J. L., Rouse, D., Bishop, S., & Johnston, S. (1997). Thought suppression, catastrophizing and pain. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21, 555–568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Sullivan, M. J. L., Thorn, B., Haythornthwaite, J. A., Keefe, F., Martin, M., Bradley, L. A., & Lefebvre, J. C. (2001). Theoretical perspectives on the relation between catastrophizing and pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, 17, 52–64.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Valet, M., Sprenger, T., Boecker, H., Willoch, F., Rummery, E., Conrad, B., Erhard, P., & Tolle, T. R. (2004). Distraction modulates connectivity of the cingulo-frontal cortex and the midbrain during pain: an fMRI analysis. Pain, 109, 399–408.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Van Damme, S., Crombez, G., & Eccleston, C. (2002). Retarded disengagement from pain cues: the effects of pain catastrophizing and pain expectancy. Pain, 100, 111–118.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Van Damme, S., Crombez, G., & Eccelston, C. (2004). The anticipation of pain modulates spatial attention: evidence for pain-specificity in high-pain catastrophizers. Pain, 111, 392–399.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Vlaeyen, J. W. S., & Linton, S. J. (2000). Fear–avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: a state of the art. Pain, 85, 317–32.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Phillip J. Quartana
    • 1
  • John W. Burns
    • 1
  • Kenneth R. Lofland
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyRosalind Franklin University of Medicine and ScienceNorth ChicagoUSA
  2. 2.Pain and Rehabilitation Clinic of ChicagoChicagoUSA

Personalised recommendations