Advertisement

Depression, Confidence, and Decision: Evidence Against Depressive Realism

  • Tiffany Fu
  • Wilma Koutstaal
  • Cynthia H. Y. Fu
  • Lucia Poon
  • Anthony J. Cleare
Article

Abstract

This research examined how retrospective self-assessments of performance are affected by major depression. To test the validity of the depressive realism versus the selective processing hypotheses, aggregate posttest performance estimates (PTPEs) were obtained from clinically depressed patients and an age-matched comparison group across 4 decision tasks (object recognition, general knowledge, social judgment, and line-length judgment). As expected on the basis of previous findings, both groups were underconfident in their PTPEs, consistently underestimating the percentage of questions they had answered correctly. Contrary to depressive realism, and in partial support of the selective processing account, this underconfidence effect was not reduced but modestly exacerbated in the depressed patients. Further, whereas the PTPEs of the comparison group exceeded that expected on the basis of chance alone those of the depressed individuals did not. The results provide no support for the depressive realism account and suggest that negative biases contribute to metacognitive information processing in major depression.

Key Words

depressive realism overconfidence underconfidence bias cognitive therapy 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (1981). Depression, non-depression, and cognitive illusions: Reply. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 436–447.Google Scholar
  2. Ackermann, R., & DeRubeis, R. J. (1991). Is depressive realism real? Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 565–584.Google Scholar
  3. Albright, J. S., & Henderson, M. C. (1995). How real is depressive realism? A question of scales and standards. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 589–609.Google Scholar
  4. Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441–485.Google Scholar
  5. Alloy, L. B., & Ackerman, L. Y. (1988). Depressive realism: Four theoretical perspectives. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.), Cognitive process in depression (pp. 223–265). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  6. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., DSM-IV). Washington, DC: APA Press.Google Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test, revised version: A study with normal adults and adults with Asperger Syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 241–251.Google Scholar
  8. Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  9. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  10. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. E., & Erbaugh, J. K. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Brenner, L. A., Koehler, D. J., Liberman, V., & Tversky, A. (1996). Overconfidence in probability and frequency judgments: A critical examination. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 212–219.Google Scholar
  12. Brunswik, E. (1943). Organismic achievement and environmental probability. Psychological Review, 50, 255–272.Google Scholar
  13. Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Alford, B. A. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy of depression. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  14. Cohen, J., Macwhinny, B., Flatt, M., & Provost, J. (1993). PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 25, 257–271.Google Scholar
  15. Colin, M. (1998). The character of mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Crawford, J. D., & Stankov, L. (1996). Age differences in the realism of confidence judgments: A calibration study using tests of fluid and crystallized intelligence. Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 83–103.Google Scholar
  17. Dobson, K. S., & Pusch, D. (1995). A test of the depressive realism hypothesis in clinically depressed subjects. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 179–194.Google Scholar
  18. Dykman, B. M., & Abramson, L. Y. (1989). Processing of ambiguous and unambiguous feedback by depressed and nondepressed college students: Schematic biases and their implications for depressive realism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 431–445.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1996). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Clinical Version (SCID-CV). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.Google Scholar
  20. Gigerenzer, G., Hoffrage, U., & Kleinbölting, H. (1991). Probabilistic mental models: A Brunswikian theory of confidence. Psychological Review, 98, 506–528.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Gotlib, I. H. (1981). Self-reinforcement and recall: Differential deficits in depressed and non-depressed psychiatric patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 521–530.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Gotlib, I. H. (1983). Perception and recall of interpersonal feedback: Negative bias in depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 7, 399–412.Google Scholar
  23. Griffin, D., & Tversky, A. (1992). The weighting of evidence and the determinants of confidence. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 411–435.Google Scholar
  24. Haaga, D. A. F., & Beck, A. T. (1995). Perspectives on depressive realism: Implications for cognitive theory of depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 41–48.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 23, 56–62.Google Scholar
  26. Hancock, J. A., Moffoot, A. P. R., & O’Carroll, R. E. (1996). “Depressive realism” assessed via confidence in decision-making. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1, 213–220.Google Scholar
  27. Johnson, T. J., & DiLorenzo, T. M. (1998). Social information processing biases in depressed and nondepressed college students. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 13, 517–530.Google Scholar
  28. Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., & Ingram, R. E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding use of the Beck Depression Inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289–299.Google Scholar
  29. Koenig, L. J., Ragin, A. B., & Harrow, M. (1995). Accuracy and bias in depressives judgements for self and other. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 505–517.Google Scholar
  30. Koutstaal, W. (2003). Older adults encode—but do not always use—perceptual details: Intentional versus unintentional effects of detail on memory judgments. Psychological Science, 14, 189–193.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Koutstaal, W., & Matthews, S. C. (2004). The relation between confidence and decision accuracy across task domains in older versus younger adults. Manuscript under revision.Google Scholar
  32. Lichtenstein, S., Fischhoff, B., & Phillips, L. D. (1982). Calibration of subjective probabilities: The state of the art up to 1980. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 306–334). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  33. McKendree-Smith, N., & Scogin, F. (2000). Depressive realism: Effects of depression severity and interpretation time. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56, 1601–1608.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Morris, S. J. (1996). Processing strategies used by disphoric individuals: Self-derogating, non-self-enhancing, or schematic? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 20, 213–233.Google Scholar
  35. Nierenberg, A. A., & DeCecco, L. M. (2001). Definitions of antidepressant treatment response, remission, nonresponse, partial response, and other relevant outcomes: A focus on treatment-resistant depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 62, 5–9.Google Scholar
  36. Pacini, R., Muir, F., & Epstein, S. (1998). Depressive realism from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1056–1068.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Sniezek, J. A., Paese, P. W., & Switzer, F. S. (1990). The effect of choosing on confidence in choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 46, 264–282.Google Scholar
  38. Stankov, L. (1998). Calibration curves, scatterplots and the distinction between general knowledge and perceptual tasks. Learning and Individual Differences, 10, 29–50.Google Scholar
  39. Stone, E. R., Dodrill, C. L., & Johnson, N. (2001). Depressive cognition: A test of depressive realism versus negativity using general knowledge questions. The Journal of Psychology, 135, 583–602.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science + Business Media, Inc. 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  • Tiffany Fu
    • 1
  • Wilma Koutstaal
    • 1
    • 2
  • Cynthia H. Y. Fu
    • 3
    • 4
  • Lucia Poon
    • 3
  • Anthony J. Cleare
    • 3
    • 4
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of ReadingReadingUnited Kingdom
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of MinnesotaMinneapolis
  3. 3.Affective Disorders UnitBethlem Royal HospitalBeckenhamUnited Kingdom
  4. 4.Institute of Psychiatry, King’s CollegeUniversity of LondonUnited Kingdom

Personalised recommendations