Journal of Logic, Language and Information

, Volume 17, Issue 4, pp 467–487 | Cite as

Discourse Processing in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

  • Michiel van Lambalgen
  • Claudia van Kruistum
  • Esther Parigger
Article

Abstract

ADHD is a psychiatric disorder characterised by persistent and developmentally inappropriate levels of inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity. It is known that children with ADHD tend to produce incoherent discourses, e.g. by narrating events out of sequence. Here the aetiology of ADHD becomes of interest. One prominent theory is that ADHD is an executive function disorder, showing deficiencies of planning. Given the close link between planning, verb tense and discourse coherence postulated in van Lambalgen and Hamm (The proper treatment of events, 2004), we predicted specific deviations in the verb tenses produced by children with ADHD. Here we report on an experiment corroborating these predictions.

Keywords

ADHD Verb tense Planning Discourse models 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aarssen J. (1996) Relating events in two languages. Acquisition of cohesive devices by Turkish-Dutch bilingual children at school age. Tilburg University Press, Tilburg, NetherlandsGoogle Scholar
  2. Baggio, G. (to appear). A semantic approach to the electrophysiology of tense. Language Learning.Google Scholar
  3. Berman, R.A., Slobin, D.I. (eds) (1994) Relating events in narrative. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.JGoogle Scholar
  4. Blankenstijn, C., & Scheper, A. (2003). Language development in children with psychiatric impairment. Published version of PhD thesis, LOT, Utrecht.Google Scholar
  5. Doets K. (1994) From logic to logic programming. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  6. Kowalski R.A. (1979) Logic for problem solving, volume 7 of Artificial Intelligence Series. North-Holland, New York and OxfordGoogle Scholar
  7. Kowalski, R. A. (1995). Using meta-logic to reconcile reactive with rational agents. In Meta-logics and logic programming. pp. 227–242. MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  8. Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? Dial books for young readers. New York.Google Scholar
  9. Ozonoff S., Strayer D.L., McMahon W.M., Filloux F. (1994) Executive function abilities in children with autism and tourette syndrom: an information-processing approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 35: 1015–1032CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Pennington B.F., Ozonoff S. (1996) Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 37: 51–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Pijnacker, J., Geurts, B., van Lambalgen, M., Buitelaar, J., Kan, C., & Hagoort, P. (2007). Conditional reasoning in high-functioning adults with autism (Submitted).Google Scholar
  12. Purvis K.L., Tannock R. (1997) Language abilities in children with attention deficit disorder, reading disabilities, and normal controls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 25(2): 133–144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Roelofs M. (1998) “Hoe bedoel je?” De verwerving van pragmatische vaardigheden. Holland Academic Graphics, The Hague, NetherlandsGoogle Scholar
  14. Russell J. (2002) Cognitive theories of autism. In: Harrison J.E., Owen A.M.(eds) Cognitive deficits in brain disorders. Dunitz, London, pp 295–323Google Scholar
  15. Shue K.L., Douglas V.I. (1992) Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and the frontal lobe syndrome. Brain and cognition 20(1): 104–124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Steedman, M. (1997). Temporality. In J. van Benthem, & A. ter Meulen (Eds.), Handbook of Logic and Language.(pp. 895–938 Chap. 16). Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
  17. Stenning, K., & van Lambalgen, M. (2007). Logic in the study of psychiatric disorders: executive function and rule-following. Topoi, 26(1), 97–114. Special issue on Logic and Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
  18. Trabasso, T., & Stein, N. L. (1994). Using goal-plan knowledge to merge the past with the present and future in narrating events on line. In M. H. Haith, J. B. Benson, R. J. Roberts, & B. F. Pennington (Eds.), The development of future-oriented processes, (pp. 323–352). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  19. van der Meulen, E. M. (2003). Attention-deficit hypercativity disorder in Dutch children. A family study on genotype, phenotype and environment. Wageningen, Netherlands: Ponsen & Looijen.Google Scholar
  20. van Lambalgen M., Hamm F. (2004) The proper treatment of events. Blackwell, Oxford and BostonGoogle Scholar
  21. Willcutt E., Doyle A.E., Nigg J.T., Faraone S.V., Pennington B.F. (2005) Validity of the executive function theory of attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry 57: 1336–1346CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Michiel van Lambalgen
    • 1
  • Claudia van Kruistum
    • 1
  • Esther Parigger
    • 2
  1. 1.Institute for Logic, Language and ComputationUniversity of AmsterdemAmsterdamThe Netherlands
  2. 2.Amsterdam Centre for Language and CommunicationUniversity of AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations