Advertisement

Journal for General Philosophy of Science

, Volume 48, Issue 2, pp 257–287 | Cite as

Kuhn Meets Maslow: The Psychology Behind Scientific Revolutions

  • Boris Kožnjak
Article

Abstract

In this paper, I offer a detailed reconstruction and a critical analysis of Abraham Maslow’s neglected psychological reading of Thomas Kuhn’s famous dichotomy between ‘normal’ and ‘revolutionary’ science, which Maslow briefly expounded four years after the first edition of Kuhn’s The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, in his small book The Psychology of Science: A Reconnaissance (1966), and which relies heavily on his extensive earlier general writing in the motivational and personality psychology. Maslow’s Kuhnian ideas, put forward as part of a larger program for the psychology of science, outlined already in his 1954 magnum opus Motivation and Personality, are analyzed not only in the context of Kuhn’s original ‘psychologizing’ attitude toward understanding the nature and development of science, but also in a broader historical, intellectual and social context.

Keywords

Thomas Kuhn Abraham Maslow Normal science Revolutionary science Psychology of science Philosophy of science 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to Gregory Feist for his interest in my work on the Maslovian Kuhn and to his valuable comments and advices, to Steve Fuller for precious bits of information about the relationship between Kuhn and Maslow, especially for informing me about the existence of the Maslow-Kuhn correspondence and for discussing its content and context, to Myles Crowley of the MIT Institute Archives and Special Collections for his complaisant assistance in accessing the mentioned correspondence in the Thomas S. Kuhn Papers, to Pavel Gregorić for his careful reading of the manuscript and his suggestions, and, last but not least, to two anonymous reviewers for providing constructive comments and suggestions that have essentially improved the quality of the paper. This work has been fully supported by the Croatian Science Foundation under the project number 5343.

References

  1. Adler, A. (1927). Understanding human nature. New York: Greenburg.Google Scholar
  2. Allchin, D. (2003). Scientific myth-conceptions. Science Education, 87(3), 329–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Andersen, H. P., Barker, P., & Chen, X. (2006). The cognitive structure of scientific revolutions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Andresen, J. (1999). Crisis and Kuhn. Isis, 90, S43–S67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barnes, B. (1974). Scientific knowledge and sociological theory. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  6. Barnes, B., & Shapin, S. (1979). Natural order: Historical studies of scientific culture. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  7. Bird, A. (2002). Kuhn’s wrong turning. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 33, 443–463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bird, A. (2012). What can cognitive science tell us about scientific revolutions? Theoria, 75, 293–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bloor, D. (1976). Knowledge and social imagery. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  10. Bloor, D. (1983). Wittgenstein: A social theory of knowledge. New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brewster Smith, M. (1966). An Ambiguous case for humanistic psychology. Science, 153(3733), 284–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bunge, M. (1998). Social science under debate: A philosophical perspective. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bunge, M. (1999). The sociology-philosophy connection. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
  14. Bush, M. (1969). Psychoanalysis and scientific creativity: With special reference to regression in the service of the ego. Journal of American Psychoanalytic Association, 17(1), 136–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cantor, G. (1999). Boyling over: A commentary on the preceding papers. British Journal for the History of Science, 32, 315–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Capaldi, E. J., & Proctor, R. W. (2014). Postmodernism and the development of the psychology of science. In G. J. Feist & M. E. Gorman (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of science (pp. 331–351). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  17. Chedd, G. (1971). Romantic at reason’s court. New Scientist and Science Journal, 49(741), 484–486.Google Scholar
  18. Driver-Linn, E. (2003). Where is psychology going? Structural fault lines revealed by psychologists’ use of Kuhn. American Psychologist, 58(4), 269–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Durkheim, E. (1982/1895). The rules of sociological method. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
  20. Ellis, A. (1967). Review of Abraham Maslow’s ‘the psychology of science: A reconnaissance’. Salmagundi, 2(1), 97–101.Google Scholar
  21. Feist, G. J. (2006). The psychology of science and the origins of the scientific mind. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Feist, G. J. (2011). Psychology of science as a new subdiscipline in psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(5), 330–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Forrester, J. (2007). On Kuhn’s case: Psychoanalysis and the paradigm. Critical Inquiry, 33, 782–819.Google Scholar
  24. Fuller, S. (2000). Thomas Kuhn: A philosophical history for our times. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  25. Fuller, S. (2013). History of the psychology of science. In G. J. Feist & M. E. Gorman (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of science (pp. 21–45). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  26. Galileo, G. (1957/1623). The assayer. In: S. Drake (Ed.), Discoveries and opinions of galileo (pp. 231–280). New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  27. Gholson, B., Shadish, W. R., Neimeyer, R. A., & Houts, A. C. (1989). Psychology of science: Contributions to metascience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gross, P., & Levitt, N. (1994). Higher superstition: The academic left and its quarrels with science. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  31. Harlow, H. (1972). Reflections on Abraham Maslow. Unpublished manuscript. Maslow Papers, Archives of the History of American Psychology, University of Akron.Google Scholar
  32. Harman, W. W. (1966). Review of Abraham Maslow’s ‘the psychology of science: A reconnaissance’. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 6, 178–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Harris, J. (1967). Review of Abraham Maslow’s ‘the psychology of science: A reconnaissance’. Canadian Journal of Counselling and Psychotherapy, 1(1), 92.Google Scholar
  34. Hoffman, E. (1988). The right to be human: A biography of Abraham Maslow. New York: St. Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
  35. Holton, G. (1993). Science and anti-science. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Horgan, J. (1991). Profile: reluctant revolutionary: Thomas S. Kuhn unleashed ‘paradigm’ on the world. Scientific American, 264(5), 40–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hornstein, G. A. (1992). The return of the repressed: Psychology’s problematic relations with psychoanalysis, 1909–1960. American Psychologist, 47, 254–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Hoyningen-Huene, P. (1987). Context of discovery and context of justification. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 18, 501–515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kubie, L. S. (1953). Some unsolved problems of the scientific career: Part I. American Scientist, 41, 596–614.Google Scholar
  40. Kubie, L. S. (1961). Neurotic distortion of the creative process. New York: Noonday Press.Google Scholar
  41. Kuhn, T. S. (1963). The function of dogma in scientific research. In A. C. Crombie (Ed.), Scientific change (pp. 347–369). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  42. Kuhn, T. S. (1970/1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  43. Kuhn, T. S. (1970b). Logic of discovery or psychology of research? In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge (pp. 1–23). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kuhn, T. S. (1977). The essential tension. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  45. Kuhn, T. S. (2000). A Discussion with Thomas S. Kuhn. In J. Conant & J. Haugeland (Eds.), The road since Structure (pp. 255–323). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  46. Laudan, L. (1996). Beyond positivism and relativism: Theory, method, and evidence. Boulder: Westview Press.Google Scholar
  47. Mahoney, M. J. (1979). Psychology of the scientist: An evaluative review. Social Studies of Science, 38, 159–160.Google Scholar
  48. Margolis, H. (1993). Paradigms and barriers. How habits of mind govern scientific beliefs. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  49. Maslow, A. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50, 370–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Maslow, A. (1946). Problem-centering vs. means-centering in science. Philosophy of Science, 13, 326–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Maslow, A. (1956). Toward a humanistic psychology. ETC: A Review of General Semantics, 14, 10–22.Google Scholar
  52. Maslow, A. (1968/1962). Toward a psychology of being. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
  53. Maslow, A. (1969/1966). The psychology of science: A reconnaissance. South Bend, Indiana: Gateway Editions.Google Scholar
  54. Maslow, A. (1970/1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  55. Maslow, A. (1971/1965). Science and self-actualization. In H. Geiger (Ed.), The Manas reader (pp. 91–99). New York: Grossman Publishers.Google Scholar
  56. Maslow, A. (1993/1971). Farther reaches of human nature. New York: Penguin/Arkana.Google Scholar
  57. Maslow, A. (1996). Defining the American Dream. In E. L. Hoffman (Ed.), Future visions: The unpublished papers of Abraham Maslow (pp. 141–146). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
  58. Maslow, A., & Mittelmann, B. (1941). Principles of abnormal psychology: The dynamics of psychic illness. New York: Harper and Brothers.Google Scholar
  59. McCall, R. J. (1967). Difficulties with humanistic science. Journal of Individual Psychology, 23(2), 240–242.Google Scholar
  60. McDonagh, L. E. (1976). Attitude changes and paradigm shifts: Social psychological foundations of Kuhnian thesis. Social Studies of Science, 6(1), 51–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Morawski, J. G., & Hornstein, G. A. (1991). Quandary of the quacks: The struggle for expert knowledge in American psychology, 1890–1940. In D. van Keuren & J. Brown (Eds.), The estate of social knowledge (pp. 106–133). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  62. Murphy, G. (1967). Review of Abraham Maslow’s ‘the psychology of science: A reconnaissance’. Contemporary Psychology, 12, 241–242.Google Scholar
  63. Nersessian, N. (2003). Kuhn, conceptual change, and cognitive science. In T. Nickles (Ed.), Thomas Kuhn (pp. 178–211). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  64. Nicholson, I. (2001). ’Giving up maleness’: Abraham Maslow, masculinity and the boundaries of psychology. History of Psychology, 4(1), 79–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Nickels, T. (2003). Normal science: From logic to case-based and model-based reasoning. In T. Nickles (Ed.), Thomas Kuhn (pp. 142–177). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  66. Pickren, W. E., & Rutherford, A. (2010). A history of modern psychology in context. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  67. Popper, K. (1970). Normal science and its dangers. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the Growth of knowledge (pp. 51–58). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2012). Psychology of science: Implicit and explicit processes. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Rorty, R. (1995). Untruth and consequences. The New Republic, 213(5), 32–36.Google Scholar
  70. Roszak, T. (1969). The making of a counter culture: Reflections of the technocratic society and its youthful opposition. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  71. Roszak, T. (1971). A unique book. New Scientist and Science Journal, 50(747), 179.Google Scholar
  72. Scharfstein, B.-A. (1980). The philosophers: Their lives and the nature of their thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  73. Schultz, D., & Schultz, S. (2011). A history of modern psychology. Boston: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
  74. Shadish, W. R., & Fuller, S. (1994). The social psychology of science. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  75. Simonton, D. K. (1988). Scientific genius: A psychology of science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  76. Stark, S. (1968). On the confounding of creativity contexts: Maslow’s psychology of science. Psychological Reports, 23(1968), 88–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Toulmin, S. (1972). The historical background to the anti-science movement. In Ciba Foundation (Ed.), Symposium 1—Civilization and science: In conflict or Collaboration? (pp. 23–32). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  78. Valiunas, A. (2011). Abraham Maslow and the all-American self. The New Atlantis, Number 33, Fall, 93–110.Google Scholar
  79. Walsh, R. T. G., Teo, T., & Baydala, A. (2014). A critical history and philosophy of psychology: Diversity of context, thought, and practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  80. Watkins, J. (1970). Against ‘normal science’. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge (pp. 25–37). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Watson, D. L. (1938). Scientists are human. London: Watts & Co.Google Scholar
  82. Wilson, J. R. (1971). Review of Abraham Maslow’s ‘the psychology of science: A reconnaissance’. New Scientist and Science Journal, 50(745), 51–52.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute of PhilosophyZagrebCroatia

Personalised recommendations