Journal of Child and Family Studies

, Volume 21, Issue 5, pp 845–856 | Cite as

Anger and Children’s Socioemotional Development: Can Parenting Elicit a Positive Side to a Negative Emotion?

  • Rachel A. Razza
  • Anne Martin
  • Jeanne Brooks-Gunn
Original Paper

Abstract

This study examined the role of anger in infancy and its interaction with maternal warmth in predicting children’s socioemotional development. Participants included a demographically diverse sample of 316 mothers and children from the Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods (PHDCN) study. Infants were followed across 3 waves of data collection from birth through 5 years of age. Mothers reported on infant anger when children were approximately 4 months of age. Maternal warmth was assessed via observation at both 4 months and 2 years. Children’s socioemotional outcomes were assessed at age 5, and included a direct assessment of delay of gratification and maternal reports of internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Anger in infancy significantly predicted higher levels of internalizing and externalizing behaviors at age 5. A main effect of anger on delay of gratification was not supported. However, anger in infancy moderated the association between maternal warmth and delay of gratification, such that only high-anger infants benefited from high maternal warmth. Similar interactive effects were not supported for problem behaviors. These results provide modest support for the differential susceptibility hypothesis, which proposes that highly reactive children are more susceptible to environmental risks and assets than other children. Specifically, findings suggest that although anger can increase children’s vulnerability to problem behaviors, it can also be a motivating factor for self-regulation in the presence of supportive parenting.

Keywords

Anger Maternal warmth Delay of gratification Problem behavior Differential susceptibility 

References

  1. Achenbach, T. (1991). Manual for the child behavior checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
  2. Allison, P. D. (2009). Missing data. In R. E. Millsap & A. Maydeu-Olivares (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of quantitative methods in psychology (pp. 72–89). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.Google Scholar
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th edn). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  4. Bates, J. E. (1989). Applications of temperament concepts. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 3–26). Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
  5. Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to rearing influence: An evolutionary argument. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 300–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Belsky, J., Hsieh, K., & Crnic, K. (1998). Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys’ externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing experience? Development and Psychopathology, 10, 301–319.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Berkowitz, L., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). Toward an understanding of the determinants of anger. Emotion, 4, 107–130.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Blandon, A. Y., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & O’Brien, M. (2010). Contributions of child’s physiology and maternal behavior to children’s trajectories of temperamental reactivity. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1089–1102.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., & Karrass, J. (2010). Fear and anger reactivity trajectories from 4 to 16 months: The roles of temperament, regulation, and maternal sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 46, 791–804.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brooks-Gunn, J., & Markman, L. (2005). The contribution of parenting to ethnic and racial gaps in school readiness. The Future of Children, 15, 138–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Buss, K. A., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1998). Fear and anger regulation in infancy: Effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expression. Child Development, 69, 359–374.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Cacioppo, J. T., Amaral, D. G., Blanchard, J. J., Cameron, J. L., Carter, C. S., Crews, D., et al. (2007). Social neuroscience: Progress and implications for mental health. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 99–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME)—Revised Edition. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas.Google Scholar
  15. Calkins, S. D. (2002). Does aversive behavior during toddlerhood matter? The effects of difficult temperament on maternal perceptions and behavior. Infant Mental Health Journal, 23, 381–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Campos, J., Campos, R., & Barrett, K. (1989). Emergent themes in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation. Developmental Psychology, 25, 394–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 183–204.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Degnan, K. A., Hane, A. A., Henderson, H. A., Moas, O. L., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., & Fox, N. A. (2010). Longitudinal stability of temperamental exuberance and social-emotional outcomes in early childhood. Developmental Psychology doi: 10.1037/a0021316.
  19. Denham, S. A., Lehman, E. B., Moser, M. H., & Reeves, S. L. (1995). Continuity and change in emotional components of infant temperament. Child Study Journal, 25, 289–308.Google Scholar
  20. Dennis, T. (2006). Emotional self-regulation in preschoolers: The interplay of child approach reactivity, parenting, and control capacities. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 84–97.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 633–652.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Early temperament and emotional development. In A. F. Kalverboer & A. Gramsbergen (Eds.), Handbook of brain and behaviour in human development (pp. 967–988). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
  23. Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241–273.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Morris, A. S., Fabes, R. A., Cumberland, A., Reiser, M., et al. (2003). Longitudinal relations among parental emotional expressivity, children’s regulation, and quality of socioemotional functioning. Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 3–19.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Spinrad, T. L., Cumberland, A., Liew, J., Reiser, M., et al. (2009). Longitudinal relations of children’s effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 45(4), 988–1008.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children’s effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 76, 1055–1071.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Fox, N. A. (1994). Dynamic cerebral processes underlying emotion regulation. In N. A. Fox (Ed.), Monographs of the society for research in child development: Vol. 59. The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations (pp. 152–166). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  28. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Fuligni, A. S., Han, W.-J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2004). The infant-toddler HOME in the second and third years of life. Parenting: Science and Practice, 4, 139–159.Google Scholar
  30. Gaertner, B., Spinrad, T. L., & Eisenberg, N. (2008). Attention in toddlers: Measurement, stability, and relations to negative emotion and parenting. Infant and Child Development, 17, 339–363.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gilliom, M., & Shaw, D. (2004). Codevelopment of externalizing and internalizing problems in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 313–333.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety. London, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Graziano, P. A., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2010). Maternal behaviour and children’s early emotion regulation skills differentially predict development of children’s reactive control and later effortful control. Infant and Child Development, 19, 333–353.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. J. B. (1998). Anger and prefrontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1310–1316.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Henderson, H. A., & Wachs, T. D. (2007). Temperament theory and the study of cognition-emotion interactions across development. Developmental Review, 27, 396–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hill, N. E., Bush, K. R., & Roosa, M. W. (2003). Parenting and family socialization strategies and children’s mental health: Low-income, Mexican-American, and Euro-American mothers and children. Child Development, 74, 189–204.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Izard, C. E., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1987). Perspectives on emotional development: I. Differential emotions theory of early emotional development. In J. D. Ososfsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd edn ed., pp. 494–554). Oxford, England: Wiley.Google Scholar
  38. Kagan, J., Arcus, D., Snidman, N., Feng, W. Y., Hendler, J., & Greene, S. (1994). Reactivity in infants: A cross-national comparison. Developmental Psychology, 30, 342–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kessler, R. C., & Mrozek, D. (1997). UM CIDI-short form. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan, Institute for Social Research.Google Scholar
  40. Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Development, 64, 325–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kochanska, G. (1995). Children’s temperament, mother’s discipline, and security of attachment: Multiple pathways to emerging internalization. Child Development, 66, 597–615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kochanska, G. (1997). Multiple ways to conscience for children with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 33, 228–240.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Kochanska, G. (2002). Committed compliance, moral self, and internalization: A mediational model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 339–351.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 1087–1112.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Kopp, C. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lahey, B. B., Van Hulle, C. A., Keenan, K., Rathouz, P. J., D’Onofrio, B. M., Rodgers, J. L., et al. (2008). Temperament and parenting during the first year of life predict future child conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1139–1158.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Laptook, R. S., Klein, D. N., Durbin, C. E., Hayden, E. P., Olino, T. M., & Carlson, G. (2008). Differentiation between los positive affectivity and behavioral inhibition in preschool age children: A comparison of behavioral approach in novel and non-novel contexts. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 758–767.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Lawson, K. R., & Ruff, H. A. (2004). Early attention and negative emotionality predict later cognitive and behavioural function. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 157–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Leerkes, E. M., Blankson, A. N., & O’Brien, M. (2009). Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social-emotional functioning. Child Development, 80(3), 762–775.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Leventhal, T., Martin, A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2004). The Early Childhood – Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (EC-HOME): Across five national datasets in the third and fifth year of life. Parenting: Science and Practice, 4, 161–188. Google Scholar
  52. Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W. (1990). Violation of expectancy, loss of control, and anger expressions in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 26, 745–751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., Ramsey, D. S., & Alessandri, S. M. (1992). Individual differences in anger and sad expressions during extinction: Antecedents and consequences. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 443–452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M. Heatherington & P. H. Mussen (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4: Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 1–101). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  55. McEwen, B. S. (2005). Stressed or stressed out: What is the difference? Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 30, 315–318.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Mikulincer, M. (1988). Reactance and helplessness following exposure to unsolvable problems: The effects of attributional style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 679–686.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Miner, J. L., & Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (2008). Trajectories of externalizing behavior from age 2 to age 9: Relations with gender, temperament, ethnicity, parenting, and rater. Developmental Psychology, 44, 771–786.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Morrell, J., & Murray, L. (2003). Parenting and the development of conduct disorder and hyperactive symptoms in childhood: A prospective longitudinal study from 2 months to 8 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 489–508.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Sessa, F. M., Avenevoli, S., & Essex, M. J. (2002). Temperament vulnerability and negative parenting as interacting predictors of child adjustment. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 461–471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Murphy, B., & Eisenberg, N. (1996). Provoked by a peer: Children’s anger related responses and their relation to social functioning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 103–124.Google Scholar
  61. Patterson, G. R. (1982). A social learning approach: Vol. 3. Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia.Google Scholar
  62. Paulussen-Hoogeboom, M. C., Stams, G. J. J. M., Hermanns, J. M. A., Thea, T. D., & van den Wittenboer, G. L. H. (2008). Parenting style as a mediator between children’s negative emotionality and problematic behavior in early childhood. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 169, 209–226.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Putnam, S. P., & Stifter, C. A. (2002). Development of approach and inhibition in the first year: Parallel findings from motor behavior, temperament ratings and directional cardiac response. Developmental Science, 5, 441–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Putnam, S. P., & Stifter, C. A. (2005). Behavioral approach-inhibition in toddlers: Prediction from infancy, positive and negative affective components, and relations with behavior problems. Child Development, 76, 212–226.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Raver, C. C. (1996). The relations between social contingency in mother-child interactions and 24-month-olds’ social competence. Developmental Psychology, 32, 850–859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Rodriguez, M. L., Ayduk, O., Aber, J. L., Mischel, W., Sethi, A., & Shoda, Y. (2005). A contextual approach to the development of self-regulatory competencies: The role of maternal unresponsitivity and toddlers’ negative affect in stressful situations. Social Development, 14, 136–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52, 569–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Rothbart, M. K. (1986). Longitudinal observation of infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 22, 356–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to 7 years: The children’s behavior questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 1394–1408.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th edn, pp. 99–166). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  71. Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Hershey, K. (2000). Stability of temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to parent report at 7 years. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Temperament and personality development across the life span (pp. 85–119). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  72. Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Executive attention and effortful control: Linking temperament, brain networks, and genes. Child Development Perspectives, 1, 2–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Royston, P. (2007). Multiple imputation of missing values: Further update of ice, with an emphasis on interval censoring. The Stata Journal, 7, 445–464.Google Scholar
  74. Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science, 277, 918–924.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Sanson, A., Hemphill, S. A., & Smart, D. (2004). Connections between temperament and social development: A review. Social Development, 13, 142–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Shaw, D. S., Keenan, K., & Vondra, J. I. (1994). Developmental precursors of externalizing behavior: Ages 1 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 30, 335–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Gaertner, B., Popp, T., Smith, C. L., Kupfer, A., et al. (2007). Relations of maternal socialization and toddlers’ effortful control to children’s adjustment and social competence. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1170–1186.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Stifter, C. A., Putnam, S., & Jahromi, L. (2008). Exuberant and inhibited toddlers: Stability of temperament and risk for problem behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 401–421.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. van Zeijl, J., Mesman, J., Stolk, M. N., Alink, L. R. A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., et al. (2007). Differential susceptibility to discipline: The moderating effect of child temperament on the association between maternal discipline and early childhood externalizing problems. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 626–636.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. von Hippel, P. T. (2007). Regression with missing Ys: An improved strategy for analyzing multiply imputed data. Sociological Methodology, 37, 83–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Zaslow, M. J., Weinfield, N. S., Gallagher, M., Hair, E. C., Ogawa, J. R., Egeland, B., et al. (2006). Longitudinal prediction of child outcomes from differing measures of parenting in a low-income sample. Developmental Psychology, 42, 27–37.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Rachel A. Razza
    • 1
  • Anne Martin
    • 2
  • Jeanne Brooks-Gunn
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Child and Family StudiesSyracuse UniversitySyracuseUSA
  2. 2.National Center for Children and Families, Teachers CollegeColumbia UniversityNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations