Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Unraveling the effect of extrinsic reading on reading with intrinsic motivation

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Journal of Cultural Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper, we analyze the effect of the time spent on reading for leisure (when the motivation is intrinsic) on the time devoted to reading for job-related or educational purposes (when the motivation is extrinsic). To do so, we use the Cultural Habits and Practices Survey conducted by the Ministry of Education and Culture of Spain in 2014–2015. As the main determinants of the time spent on intrinsic reading, we consider the time devoted to extrinsic reading, sociodemographic characteristics, labor situation, participation in other leisure activities and an index of cultural capital at home. We estimate a Heckman model that allows us to control for self-selection. Results show that the time devoted to intrinsic reading mainly depends on the time spent on reading with extrinsic motivation, human capital and cultural background. Reading with extrinsic motivation increases the likelihood of intrinsic reading but reduces the time allocated to it. Therefore, our results suggest a substitution pattern between time spent reading for leisure and reading with extrinsic motivation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This implies that the MRS between leisure activities j and k within leisure decision is independent of working hours.

  2. As our dependent variable, Intrinsic Reading (IR) is heavily skewed and has considerable non-normal kurtosis, we firstly estimate the λ parameter of the Box–Cox transformation in order to test if the regression model for the time devoted to reading is better in logs than in levels. Given that \(\hat{\lambda } = 0.13\), we have greater support for expressing this variable in logs (Cameron and Trivedi 2009). The same holds for Extrinsic Reading (ER), so we also take its natural logarithm (Ln_ER).

  3. We assume that physical cultural capital is highly correlated with household income.

  4. Several attempts to split PCA into two or three groups of variables were made, in order to check if different types of cultural capital could affect reading habits differently. However, the use of a single index of cultural capital provides the best model fit, so we it was the preferred option.

  5. This transformation sets the dependent variable to missing as Ƭ = 0; so, it is necessary to substitute the censoring point Ƭ by another small value close to zero but different from it. This allows ln Ƭ to exist.

  6. There have been some attempts to move away from the normality assumption (Martins 2001). Nonetheless, the empirical literature is dominated by the joint normal distribution assumption.

  7. As individuals were questioned about their reading habits within the last 3 months, we consider that when a respondent declares that he/she has not read during the considered period, we cannot deem him as an actual reader. This validates the election of the Heckman model which assumes that all the zeros are generated by the non-participation decision.

  8. As the error terms are assumed to be correlated and the structure of the model is recursive, it is recommended that at least one variable in Z does not appear in X.

  9. Estimates were conducted using the heckman module in Stata 14.

  10. When the error assumptions are met, the FIML estimator will always be more efficient than the Heckman two-step alternative (Puhani 2000).

  11. To examine the potential existence of multicollinearity, we have computed the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) after an OLS regression. All the values are below 10, which is normally taken as the threshold point, so we consider that there is no such problem in our model.

  12. To allow for possible non-linearities, squared term was introduced. Since it was not statistically significant, it was finally omitted. We also defined a set of age groups. The results of this alternative specification are consistent: Age does not explain the amount of time reading with intrinsic motivation. Results are available upon request.

  13. Since retired and those younger than 13 years old have not been considered in our sample, age range is substantially narrower than usually, which could partially explain why age issues do not arise.

  14. For the sake of parsimony, we only report the AME for ln PRW, Male, Age, PCA Cultural Capital and Cultural Participation. The rest of AME are available from the authors upon request.

References

  • Allington, R., & McGill-Franzen, A. (2003). The impact of summer setback on the reading achievement gap. Phi Delta Kappan,85(1), 68–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andreassen, R., & Bråten, I. (2010). Examining the prediction of reading comprehension on different multiple-choice tests. Journal of Research in Reading,33, 263–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S. (1964). Human capital. New York: Columbia University Press for the National Bureau of Economic Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S. (1965). A theory of the allocation of time. Economic Journal,75, 493–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S. (1996). Accounting for tastes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S., & Murphy, K. M. (1988). A theory of rational addiction. Journal of Political Economy,96(4), 675–700.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, M., McElvany, N., & Kortenbruck, M. (2010). Intrinsic and extrinsic reading motivation as predictors of reading literacy: A longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology,102(4), 773–785.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bortolussi, M., Dixon, P., & Sopcak, P. (2010). Gender and reading. Poetics,38, 299–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P., & Passeron, J. C. (1990). Reproduction in education, society and culture (2nd ed.). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bravishi, A., Slade, M. D., & Levy, B. R. (2016). A chapter a day: Association of book reading with longevity. Social Science and Medicine,164, 44–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brozko, W. C., Sulkunen, S., Shiel, G., Garbe, C., Pandian, A., & Valtin, R. (2014). Reading, gender and engagement. Lessons from five PISA countries. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy,57(7), 584–593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, A. C., & Trivedi, P. K. (2005). Microeconometrics. Methods and applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, A. C., & Trivedi, P. K. (2009). Microeconometrics using Stata. College Station: Stata Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carbonaro, W. (2007). The effects of education and cognitive skill on earnings: How much do occupations and jobs matter? Research in Social Stratification and Mobility,25, 57–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, M. M., & Chow, B. W. Y. (2010). Culture, motivation, and reading achievement: High school students in 41 countries. Learning and Individual Differences,20, 579–592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, C. (2011). Setting the baseline: The National Literacy Trust’s first annual survey into young people’s reading 2010. London: National Literacy Trust.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, C., & Foster, A. (2005). Children’s and young people’s reading habits and preferences: The who, what, why, where and when. London: National Literacy Trust.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, C., Osborne, S., & Akerman, R. (2008). Young people’s self-perceptions as readers: An investigation including family, peer and school influences. London: National Literacy Trust.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, C., & Zoysa, S. (2011). Mapping the interrelationships of reading enjoyment, attitudes, behaviour and attainment: An exploratory investigation. London: National Literacy Trust.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coles, M., & Hall, C. (2002). Gendered readings: Learning from children’s reading choices. Journal of Research in Reading,25(1), 96–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deaton, A. S., & Muellbauer, J. (1980). Economics and consumer behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elvestad, E., & Blekesaume, A. (2008). Newspaper readers in Europe. A multilevel study of individual and national differences. European Journal of Communication,23(4), 425–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Blanco, V., & Prieto-Rodríguez, J. (2009). Análisis de los hábitos de lectura como una decisión económica. Estudios de Economía Aplicada,27(1), 235–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Blanco, V., Prieto-Rodríguez, J., & Suárez-Pandiello, J. (2017). A quantitative analysis of reading habits in Spain. International Journal of Arts Management,19(3), 19–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gil-Flores, J. (2009). Hábitos y Actitudes de las Familias Hacia la Lectura y Competencias Básicas del Alumnado. Revista de Educación,350, 301–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, A. W., Schlackman, J., Gottfried, A. E., & Boutin-Martinez, A. S. (2015). Parental provision of early literacy environment as related to reading and educational outcomes across the academic lifespan. Parenting,15(1), 24–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goux, D., Gurgand, M., & Maurin, E. (2017). Reading enjoyment and reading skills: Lessons from an experiment with first grade children. Labour Economics,45, 17–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, J. T., & Davis, M. H. (2003). Motivating struggling readers in middle school through an engagement model of classroom practice. Reading & Writing Quarterly,19(1), 59–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, J. T., Hoa, A. L. W., Wigfield, A., Tonks, S. M., Humenick, N. M., & Littles, E. (2007). Reading motivation and reading comprehension growth in the later elementary years. Contemporary Educational Psychology,32(3), 282–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, J. T., & Klauda, S. L. (2012). Making textbook reading meaningful. Educational Leadership,69(6), 64–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckman, J. J. (1979). Sample selection bias as a specification error. Econometrica,47, 153–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckman, J. J., & Masterov, D. V. (2007). The productivity argument for investing in young children. Review of Agricultural Economics,29(3), 446–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hicks, J. R. (1939). Value and capital. The Economic Journal,49(194), 294–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ivey, G., & Broaddus, K. (2001). ‘Just Plain Reading’: A survey of what makes students want to read in middle school classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly,36, 350–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalb, G., & Van Ours, J. C. (2014). Reading to young children: A head-start in life? Economics of Education Review, 40, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kloosterman, R., Notten, N., Tolsma, J., & Kraaykamp, G. (2011). The effects of parental reading socialization and early school involvement on children's academic performance: A panel study of primary school pupils in the Netherlands. European Sociological Review,26(2), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knulst, W., & Kraaykamp, G. (1998). Trends in leisure reading: Forty years of research on reading in the Netherlands. Poetics,26(1), 21–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knulst, W., & Van den Broek, A. (2003). The readership of books in times of de-reading. Poetics,31, 213–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koolstra, C. M., & van der Voort, T. H. A. (1996). Longitudinal effects of television on children’s leisure-time reading: A test of three explanatory models. Human Communication Research,23, 4–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kooreman, P., & Kapteyn, A. (1987). A disaggregated analysis of the allocation of time within the household. Journal of Political Economy,95(2), 223–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lauf, E. (2001). Research note: The vanishing young reader. Sociodemographic determinants of newspaper use as source of political information in Europe, 1980–1998. European Journal of Communication,16(2), 233–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, T., Lin, C., Huang, K., & Chen, Y. (2017). Effects of noise type, noise intensity, and illumination intensity on reading performance. Applied Acoustics,120, 70–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loan, F.A. (2012). Impact of the internet surfing on reading practices and choices. Webology, 9(1), Article 94

  • Logan, S., & Johnston, R. S. (2009). Gender differences in reading: Examining where these differences lie. Journal of Research in Reading,32(2), 199–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, S., Medford, E., & Hughes, N. (2011). The importance of intrinsic motivation for high and low ability readers' reading comprehension performance. Learning and Individual Differences,21(1), 124–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maddala, G. S. (1999). Limited-dependent and qualitative variables in econometrics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martins, M. F. O. (2001). Parametric and semiparametric estimation of sample selection models: An empirical application to the female labour force in Portugal. Journal of Applied Econometrics,16(1), 23–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCain, R. (1979). Reflection on cultivation of taste. Journal of Cultural Economics,3(1), 30–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGeown, S. P. (2015). Sex or gender identity? Understanding children’s reading choices and motivation. Journal of Research in Reading,38(1), 35–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGeown, S. P., Goodwin, H., Henderson, N., & Wright, P. (2012a). Gender differences in reading motivation: Does sex or gender identity provide a better account? Journal of Research in Reading,35(3), 328–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGeown, S. P., Norgate, R., & Warhurst, A. (2012b). Exploring intrinsic and extrinsic reading motivation among very good and very poor readers. Educational Research,54(3), 309–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • McIntosh, S., & Vignoles, A. (2001). Measuring and assessing the impact of basic skills on labour market outcomes. Oxford Economics Papers,3, 453–481.

    Google Scholar 

  • McQuillan, J., & Au, J. (2001). The effect of print access on reading frequency. Reading Psychology,22, 225–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metsäpelto, R., Silinskas, G., Kiuru, N., Poikkeus, A., Pakarinen, E., Vasalampi, K., et al. (2017). Externalizing behavior problems and interest in reading as predictors of later reading skills and educational aspirations. Contemporary Educational Psychology,49, 324–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mokhtari, K., Reichard, C. A., & Gardner, A. (2009). The impact of internet and television use on the reading habits and practices of college students. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy,52(7), 609–619.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mol, S. E., & Bus, A. G. (2011). To read or not to read: A meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Psychological Bulletin,137, 267–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molina, J. A., Campaña, J. C., & Ortega, R. (2016). What do you prefer for a relaxing time at home: Reading, watching TV or listening to the radio? Applied Economic Letters,23(18), 1278–1284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, P. L., & Fuchs, D. (2007). Is there a bidirectional relationship between children's reading skills and reading motivation? Exceptional Children,73(2), 165–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullan, K. (2010). Families that read: A time diary analysis of young people’s and parents’ reading. Journal of Research in Reading,33, 414–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murnane, R. J., Willett, J. B., Duhaldeborde, Y., & Tyler, J. H. (2000). How important are the cognitive skills of teenagers in predicting subsequent earnings? Journal of Policy Analysis and Management,19(4), 547–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielen, T. M. J., Mol, S. E., Sikkema-de Jong, M. T., & Bus, A. G. (2016). Attentional bias toward reading in reluctant readers. Contemporary Educational Psychology,46, 263–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Notten, N. (2011). Parents and the media. Causes and consequences of parental media socialization. Doctoral dissertation. Nijmegen. The Netherlands: Radboud University Nijmegen.

  • Notten, N., Kraaykamp, G., & Konig, R. P. (2012). Family media matters: Unraveling the intergenerational transmission of reading and television tastes. Sociological Perspectives,55, 683–706.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfost, M., Schiefer, I. M., & Artelt, C. (2016). Intergenerational continuity in attitudes toward reading and reading behavior. Learning and Individual Differences,51, 179–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollak, R. A. (1969). Conditional demand functions and consumption theory. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 83(1), 60–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puhani, P. A. (2000). The Heckman correction for sample selection and its critique. Journal of Economic Surveys,14, 53–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Retelsdorf, J., Köller, O., & Möller, J. (2011). On the effects of motivation on reading performance growth in secondary school. Learning and Instruction,21(4), 550–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roe, A., & Taube, K. (2012). To read or not to read—That is the question. Reading engagement and reading habits in a gender perspective. In N. Egelund (Ed.), Northern lights on PISA 2009—focus on reading. Copenhagen: Nordic Council of Ministers.

  • Rosenzweig, E. Q., & Wigfield, A. (2017). What if reading is easy but unimportant? How students’ patterns of affirming and undermining motivation for reading information texts predict different reading outcomes. Contemporary Educational Psychology,48, 133–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scales, A. M., & Rhee, O. (2001). Adult reading habits and patterns. Reading Psychology,22, 175–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaffner, E., Schiefele, U., & Ulferts, H. (2013). Reading amount as a mediator of the effects of intrinsic and extrinsic reading motivation on reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly,48(4), 369–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, U., Schaffner, E., Möller, J., & Wigfield, A. (2012). Dimensions of reading motivation and their relation to reading behavior and competence. Reading Research Quarterly,47(4), 427–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, U., Stutz, F., & Schaffner, E. (2016). Longitudinal relations between reading motivation and reading comprehension in the early elementary grades. Learning and Individual Differences,51, 49–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutte, N. S., & Malouff, J. M. (2004). University student reading preferences in relation to the big five personality dimensions. Reading Psychology,25(4), 273–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siddiqi, A., Subramanian, S. V., Berkman, L., Hertzman, C., & Kawachi, I. (2007). The welfare state as a context for children’s development: A study of the effects of unemployment and unemployment protection on reading literacy scores. International Journal of Social Welfare,16, 314–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sikora, J., Evans, M. D. R., & Kelley, J. (2019). Scholarly culture: How books in adolescence enhance adult literacy, numeracy and technology skills in 31 societies. Social Science Research,77, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soemer, A., & Schiefele, U. (2019). Text difficulty, topic interest, and mind wandering during reading. Learning and Instruction,61, 12–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in understanding reading: Scientific foundations and new frontiers. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, G. J., & Becker, G. S. (1977). De Gustibus Non Est Disputandum. American Economic Review,67(2), 76–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stokmans, M. J. W. (1999). Reading attitude and its effect on leisure time reading. Poetics,26, 245–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stutz, F., Schaffner, E., & Schiefele, U. (2016). Relations among reading motivation, reading amount, and reading comprehension in the early elementary grades. Learning and Individual Differences,45, 101–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suggate, S., Schaughency, E., McAnally, H., & Reeseb, E. (2018). From infancy to adolescence: The longitudinal links between vocabulary, early literacy skills, oral narrative, and reading. Cognitive Development,47, 82–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tepper, S. (2000). Fiction reading in America: Explaining the gender gap. Poetics,27, 255–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torppa, M., Eklund, K., Sulkunen, S., Niemi, P., & Ahonen, T. (2018). Why do boys and girls perform differently on PISA Reading in Finland? The effects of reading fluency, achievement behaviour, leisure Reading and homework activity. Journal of Research in Reading,41(1), 122–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Troyer, M., Kim, J. S., Hale, E., Wantchekon, K. A., & Armstrong, C. (2019). Relations among intrinsic and extrinsic reading motivation, reading amount, and comprehension: A conceptual replication. Reading and Writing,32, 1197–1218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verboord, M. (2005). Long-term effects of literary education on book-reading frequency: An analysis of Dutch student cohorts 1975–1998. Poetics,33, 320–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verboord, M., & Van Rees, K. (2003). Do changes in socialization lead to a decline in reading level? How parents, literary education, and popular culture affect the level of books read. Poetics,31, 283–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verghese, J., Lipton, R. B., Katz, M. J., Hall, C. B., Derby, C. A., Kuslansky, G., et al. (2003). Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. The New England Journal of Medicine,348, 2508–2516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, J. H. Y., & Guthrie, J. T. (2004). Modeling the effects of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, amount of reading, and past reading achievement on text comprehension between US and Chinese students. Reading Research Quarterly,39, 162–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M. T., & Eccles, J. S. (2013). School context, achievement motivation, and academic engagement: A longitudinal study of school engagement using a multidimensional perspective. Learning and Instruction,28, 12–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wollscheid, S. (2014). The impact of the leisure reading behaviors of both parents on children’s reading behavior: Investigating differences between sons and daughters. Poetics,45, 36–54.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. All errors in fact or interpretation are ours. We would like to thank Víctor Fernández-Blanco and Juan Prieto-Rodríguez, the editor and two anonymous referees for their constructive comments

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sara Suárez-Fernández.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Principal component analysis for cultural capital

Following the practice of Fernández-Blanco and Prieto-Rodríguez (2009), we conducted a PCA for cultural capital in order to proxy household income. We included the number of books, e-books, encyclopedias, vinyl CDs, DVDs, CDs and Blue Rays, and other audio devices, as well as the number of computers. Descriptive statistics of each variable can be seen on Table 5.

Table 5 Principal component analysis (cultural capital) (N = 10,319)

The coefficient for the first factor is positive for all considered variables and, consequently, first factor analysis predicts that the cultural capital is positively correlated with the variables that we contemplate. The eigenvalue of the first factor is 1.025, and it explains the 62.68% of the total variance.

Appendix 2: Heckman estimates by labor status

See Table 6.

Table 6 Estimation results for intrinsic reading (Heckman) by labor status

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Suárez-Fernández, S., Boto-García, D. Unraveling the effect of extrinsic reading on reading with intrinsic motivation. J Cult Econ 43, 579–605 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10824-019-09361-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10824-019-09361-4

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation