Advertisement

The rationality of literal Tide Pod consumption

  • Ryan H. MurphyEmail author
Article

Abstract

At the conclusion of 2017, to the dismay of journalists, pundits, and academics, large numbers of adolescents began consuming Tide Pods, a form of laundry detergent that is candy-like in appearance. This paper argues that purposeful consumption of laundry detergent may in fact be individually rational for adolescents. The consumption of Tide Pods may allow adolescents to successfully signal status in accordance with the Handicap Principle, which explains the beauty of a peacock’s tail and the practice of stotting by gazelles in the wild. The Handicap Principle is also a common explanation of adolescents’ willingness to engage in dangerous activities, like drug use. A subtext of the thesis of this paper is the veracity of rational choice explanations in unconventional contexts distant from its original applications.

Keywords

Tide Pods Veblen good Conspicuous consumption Handicap principle 

JEL Classification

Z13 B52 

Notes

References

  1. Abad-Santos, A. (2018). Why people are (mostly) joking about eating Tide Pods. Vox, https://www.vox.com/2018/1/4/16841674/tide-pods-eating-meme-tide-pod-challenge
  2. Alchian, A. (1950). Uncertainty, evolution, and economic theory. Journal of Political Economy, 58(3), 211–221.Google Scholar
  3. American Association of Poison Control Centers. (2018a). Laundry detergent packets (unit dose liquid) data. Retrieved October 4, 2018, from https://aapcc.s3.amazonaws.com/files/library/Laundry_Pack_Web_Data_through_3.2018.pdf.
  4. American Association of Poison Control Centers. (2018b). Intentional exposures among teens to single-load laundry packets. Retrieved October 4, 2018, from http://www.aapcc.org/alerts/intentional-exposures-among-teens-single-load-laun/. This page was deleted but has been archived: https://web.archive.org/web/20180430235815/, http://www.aapcc.org/alerts/intentional-exposures-among-teens-single-load-laun/
  5. Baker Jr, M. D., & Maner, J. K. (2008). Risk-taking as a situationally sensitive male mating strategy. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 391–395.Google Scholar
  6. Baker Jr, M. D., & Maner, J. K. (2009). Male risk-taking as a context-sensitive signaling device. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(5), 1136–1139.Google Scholar
  7. Basso, F., Robert-Demontrond, P., Hayek, M., Anton, J.-L., Nazarian, B., Roth, M., & Oullier, O. (2014). Why people drink shampoo? Food imitating products are fooling brains and endangering consumers for marketing purposes. PLoS ONE, 9(9), 1–17.Google Scholar
  8. Becker, G. (1962). Irrational behavior and economic theory. Journal of Political Economy, 70(1), 1–13.Google Scholar
  9. Becker, G., & Murphy, K. (1988). A theory of rational addiction. Journal of Political Economy, 96(4), 675–700.Google Scholar
  10. Belluz, J. (2014). The truth about the ice bucket challenge: Viral memes shouldn’t dictate our charitable giving. Vox, https://www.vox.com/2014/8/20/6040435/als-ice-bucket-challenge-and-why-we-give-to-charity-donate
  11. Bever, L. (2018). Teens are daring each other to eat Tide Pods. We don’t need to tell you that’s a bad idea. The Washington Post, https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/to-your-health/wp/2018/01/13/teens-are-daring-each-other-to-eat-tide-pods-we-dont-need-to-tell-you-thats-a-bad-idea/?utm_term=.7dae3d8c8955
  12. Boardman, A. E., Greenberg, D. H., Vining, A. R., & Weimer, D. L. (2006). Cost-benefit analysis: Concepts and practice (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  13. Borkowska, B., & Pawlowski, B. (2014). Recreational drug use and fluctuating asymmetry: Testing the handicap principle. Evolutionary Psychology, 12(4), 769–782.Google Scholar
  14. Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 367–383.Google Scholar
  15. Caplan, Bryan. (2018). The case against education: Why the education system is a waste of time and money. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Collins, J. (2017). Rationalizing the ‘irrational’. Behavioral Scientist, http://behavioralscientist.org/rationalizing-the-irrational/
  17. Collins, J., Baer, B., & Weber, E. J. (2015). Sexual selection, conspicuous consumption and economic growth. Journal of Bioeconomics, 17(2), 186–209.Google Scholar
  18. Consumer Reports News. (2013). As poisoning cases rise, a call for safer laundry pod packaging. https://www.consumerreports.org/cro/news/2013/03/as-poisoning-cases-rise-a-call-for-safer-laundry-pod-packaging/index.htm
  19. Dahir, I. (2018). Let’s kick of 2018 with a warning from tide not to eat its laundry pacs. Buzzfeed, https://www.buzzfeed.com/ikrd/the-tide-pods-wont-get-you-high?utm_term=.vuZk6ZxeAK#.uvV1Q3KBgY
  20. De Fraja, G. (2009). The origins of utility: Sexual selection and conspicuous consumption. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 72(1), 51–69.Google Scholar
  21. Diamond, Jared. (1992). The third chimpanzee: The evolution and future of the human animal. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
  22. Durrant, Russell, Adamson, Simon, Todd, Fraser, & Selmman, Doug. (2009). Drug use and addiction: Evolutionary perspective. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 43(11), 1049–1056.Google Scholar
  23. Farthing, G. William. (2005). Attitudes toward heroic and nonheroic physical risk takers as mates and as friends. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(2), 171–185.Google Scholar
  24. Frank, R. H. (2005). Departures from rational choice: With and without regret. In Francesco Parisi & Vernon L. Smith (Eds.), The law and economics of irrational behavior. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  25. Frank, R. H. (2018). Why even tougher smoking regulations are justified. The New York Times, https://www.nytimes.com/2018/01/05/business/economy/why-even-tougher-regulations-on-smoking-are-justified.html
  26. Grant-Alfieri, A., Schaechter, J., & Lipshultz, S. E. (2013). Ingesting and aspirating dry cinnamon by children and adolescents: the ‘cinnamon challenge’. Pediatrics, 131(5), 833–835.Google Scholar
  27. Greenwood, J., Guner, N., Kocharkov, G., & Santos, C. (2014). Marry your like: Assortive mating and income inequality. American Economic Review, 105(4), 348–353.Google Scholar
  28. Greitemeyer, T., Kastemuller, A., & Fischer, P. (2013). Romantic motives and risk-taking: An evolutionary approach. Journal of Risk Research, 16(1), 19–38.Google Scholar
  29. Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Delton, A. W., & Robertson, T. E. (2011). The theory of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(6), 1015–1026.Google Scholar
  30. Gstalater, M. (2018). YouTube removing videos of people eating Tide Pods. The Hill, http://thehill.com/policy/technology/369531-youtube-facebook-removing-videos-of-people-eating-tide-pods
  31. Hagen, E. H., Roulette, C. J., & Sullivan, R. J. (2013). Explaining human recreational use of ‘pesticides’: The neurotoxin regulation model of substance use vs. the hijack model and implications for age and sex differences in drug consumption. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4(Article 142), 1–21.Google Scholar
  32. Hoffer, S., Beradino, F., Smith, J., & Rubin, S. (1998). Economic values for evaluation of FAA investment and regulatory decisions. Washington, DC: Federal Aviation Administration.Google Scholar
  33. Huntington, S., Heppner, J., Vohra, R., Mallois, R., & Geller, R. J. (2014). Serious adverse effects from single-use detergent sacs: Report from a U.S. Statewide Poison Control System. Clinical Toxicology, 52, 220–225.Google Scholar
  34. Ioannidis, J. P. A., Stanley, T. D., & Doucouliagos, H. (2017). The power of bias in economics research. The Economic Journal, 127(605), F236–F265.Google Scholar
  35. Karel, L. I., Handzel, M. C., & Rosini, J. M. (2015). Laundry detergent pod ingestion in 2 pediatric patients. Journal of Emergency Nursing, 41(1), 80–82.Google Scholar
  36. Kavka, G. S. (1983). The toxin puzzle. Analysis, 43(1), 33–36.Google Scholar
  37. Kelly, S., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2001). Who dares, wins. Human Nature, 12(2), 89–105.Google Scholar
  38. Kenrick, D. T., & Griskevicius, V. (2013). The rational animal: How evolution made us smarter than we think. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  39. KnowYourMeme. (2018). Tide POD challenge.” http://knowyourmeme.com/memes/tide-pod-challenge
  40. Leeson, P. T. (2012). Ordeals. Journal of Law and Economics, 55(3), 691–714.Google Scholar
  41. Leeson, P. T. (2017). WTF?! An economic tour of the weird. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Leeson, P. T., & Coyne, C. J. (2012). Sassywood. Journal of Comparative Economics, 40, 608–620.Google Scholar
  43. Levitt, S. D., & Venkatesh, S. A. (2000). An economic analysis of a drug-selling gang’s finances. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115(3), 755–789.Google Scholar
  44. Murphy, R. H. (2016). The willingness-to-pay for caplanian irrationality. Rationality and Society, 28(1), 52–82.Google Scholar
  45. Popper, K. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson Co.Google Scholar
  46. Redford, A. (2017). Don’t eat the brown acid: Induced ‘malnovation’ in drug markets. Review of Austrian Economics, 30(2), 215–233.Google Scholar
  47. Ridley, M. (1996). The origins of virtue: human instincts and the evolution of cooperation. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
  48. Ronay, R., & von Hippel, W. (2010). The presence of an attractive woman elevates testosterone and physical risk taking in young men. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1(1), 57–64.Google Scholar
  49. Rubin, P. (2003). Folk economics. Southern Economic Journal, 70(1), 157–171.Google Scholar
  50. Rubin, P., & Paul, C. W. (1979). An evolutionary model of taste for risk. Economic Inquiry, 17(4), 585–596.Google Scholar
  51. Shanks, D. R., Vadillo, M. A., Riedel, B., Clymo, A., Govind, S., Hickin, N., Tamman, A. J. F., & Puhlmann, L. M. C. (2015). Romance, risk, and replication: can consumer choices and risk-taking be primed by mating motives? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(6), e142–e158.Google Scholar
  52. Sjogren, P. P., Skarda, D. E., & Park, A. H. (2016). Upper aerodigestive injuries from detergent ingestion in children. The Laryngoscope, 127(February), 509–512.Google Scholar
  53. Smith, E., Liebelt, E., & Nogueira, J. (2014). Laundry detergent pod ingestions: Is there a need for endoscopy? Journal of Medical Toxicology, 10(3), 286–291.Google Scholar
  54. Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Development Review, 28(1), 78–106.Google Scholar
  55. Stigler, G., & Becker, G. (1977). De gustibus non est disputandum. American Economic Review, 67(2), 76–90.Google Scholar
  56. Stromberg, P. E., Burt, M. H., Rose, S. R., Cumpston, K. L., Emswiler, M. P., & Wills, B. K. (2015). Airway compromise in children exposed to single-use laundry detergent pods: A poison center observational case series. American Journal of Emergency Medicine, 33, 349–351.Google Scholar
  57. Sundie, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Vohs, K. D., & Beal, D. J. (2010). Peacocks, porsches, and thorstein veblen: Conspicuous consumption as a sexual signaling system. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(4), 664–680.Google Scholar
  58. Tullock, G. (1962). The welfare costs of tariffs, monopolies, and theft. Economic Inquiry, 5(2), 224–232.Google Scholar
  59. Valdez, A. L., Casavant, M. J., Spiller, H. A., Chounthirath, T., Xiang, H., & Smith, G. A. (2014). Pediatric exposure to laundry pods. Pediatrics, 134(6), 1127–1135.Google Scholar
  60. Veblen, T. (1899). The theory of the leisure class. Abingdon: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  61. Vincke, E. (2017). The deep rationality of dark consumption: Alcohol and cigarette use as sexual signaling. Dissertation, Ghent University.Google Scholar
  62. von Mises, L. (1949). Human action. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  63. Wilke, A., Hutchinson, J. M. C., Todd, P. M., & Kruger, D. J. (2006). Is risk taking used as a cue in mate choice? Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 367–393.Google Scholar
  64. Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1985). Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: The young male syndrome. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6(1), 59–73.Google Scholar
  65. Yin, S., Behrman, A., & Colvin, J. (2015). Laundry pack exposures in children 0-5 years evaluated at a single pediatric institution. The Journal of Emergency Medicine, 48(5), 566–572.Google Scholar
  66. Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection—A selection for handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53(1), 205–214.Google Scholar
  67. Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.The O’Neil Center for Global Markets and Freedom, SMU Cox School of BusinessSouthern Methodist UniversityDallasUSA

Personalised recommendations