Advertisement

Journal of Adult Development

, Volume 24, Issue 4, pp 252–262 | Cite as

The Role of the Listener on the Emotional Valence of Personal Memories in Emerging Adulthood

  • Chiara Fioretti
  • Debora Pascuzzi
  • Andrea Smorti
Article
  • 273 Downloads

Abstract

Many scholars stressed the role of social interactions in the construction of autobiographical memories, especially in late adolescence and emerging adulthood. This paper aims to assess the impact of the listener attitude on narrator’s emotional valence of past life events concerning the end of a close relationship. 157 emerging adults have been asked to recall a memory and to randomly narrate it to a listener previously trained to be distracted and detached (DL group) versus attentive and empathic (AL group). A control group (CG) had only to reflect internally on the recalled memory. Participants had to allocate one or more emotions to their memory from a 12-item list in a recall task, a narrative/reflection task and a 15-day recall follow-up. The percentages of negative, positive and neutral emotions were assessed and changes among the three emotional allocations were measured. Results showed that participants of the AL group after the narrative task increased the positive emotional engagement of memories and decreased the negative emotions in comparison to DL participants and the CG ones. The authors interpret the results suggesting that narrating autobiographical memories to attentive peers is a way to co-construct their emotional meaning and discuss findings in the light of the knowledge on the lifespan period of emerging adulthood.

Keywords

Autobiographical memory Narrative Relation Emerging adulthood 

Notes

Acknowledgements

A special thanksgiving to Roberta Catilino who participated in the implementation of the present study. The authors are furthermore grateful to all the students who participated in the present study sharing their precious stories.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469–480. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Arnett, J. J. (2001). Conceptions of the transition to adulthood: Perspectives from adolescence through midlife. Journal of Adult Development, 8(2), 133–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Belsky, J. (2009). Experiencing the lifespan. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  4. Brans, K., Van Mechelen, I., Rimé, B., & Verduyn, P. (2013). The relation between social sharing and the duration of emotional experience. Cognition & Emotion, 27(6), 1023–1041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brans, K., Van Mechelen, I., Rimé, B., & Verduyn, P. (2014). To share, or not to share? Examining the emotional consequences of social sharing in the case of anger and sadness. Emotion (Washington, D. C.), 14(6), 1062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bruner, J. M. (1991). The narrative construction of reality. Critical Inquiry, 18(1), 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Burke, K. (1945). A grammar of motives. New York: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  8. Christianson, S. Å. (1992). The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  9. Conway, M. A. (1997). Recovered memories and false memories. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Conway, M. A., & Holmes, A. (2004). Psychosocial stages and the accessibility of autobiographical memories across the life cycle. Journal of Personality, 72(3), 461–480. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00269.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107(2), 261–288.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Farrar, M. J., Fasig, L. G., & Welch-Ross, M. K. (1997). Attachment and emotion in autobiographical memory development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67(3), 389–408. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1997.2414.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Fioretti, C., & Smorti, A. (2014). Improving doctor-patient communication through an autobiographical narrative theory. Communication & Medicine, 11(3), 275–284. doi: 10.1558/cam.v11i3.20369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fioretti, C., & Smorti, A. (2015). How emotional content of memories changes in narrative. Narrative Inquire, 25(1), 37–56. doi: 10.1075/ni.25.1.03fio.Google Scholar
  15. Fioretti, C., & Smorti, A. (2016). Narrating positive versus negative memories of illness: Does narrating influence the availability and the emotional involvement of memories of illness? European Journal of Cancer Care, 1–7. doi: 10.1111/ECC.12524.Google Scholar
  16. Fivush, R. (1998). Interest, gender and personal narrative: How children construct self-understanding. In A. Karp, A. Renninger, J. Baumesteir & L. Hoffman (Eds.), Interest and gender in education (pp. 58–73). Kiel: Institute for Science Education.Google Scholar
  17. Fivush, R. (2011). The development of autobiographical memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 559–582. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131702.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Fivush, R., & Baker-Ward, L. (2005). The search for meaning: Developmental Perspectives on internal state language in autobiographical memory. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6(4), 455–462. doi: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0604_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2004). Culture and language in the emergence of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15(9), 573–577. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00722.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Frattaroli, J. (2006). Experimental disclosure and its moderators: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 823.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Habermas, T., & Bluck, S. (2000). Getting a life: The emergence of the life story in adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 126(5), 748–769.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Habermas, T., Ehlert-Lerche, S., & De Silveira, C. (2009). The development of the temporal macrostructure of life narratives across adolescence: Beginnings, linear narrative form, and endings. Journal of Personality, 77(2), 527–560.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity and the storied self: A contemporary framework for study persons. Psychological Inquiry, 7, 295–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. McAdams, D. P., Diamond, A., De St Aubin, E., & Mansfield, E. (1997). Stories of commitment: The psychosocial construction of generative lives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 678–694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. McAdams, D. P., Reynolds, J., Lewis, M., Patten, A. H., & Bowman, P. J. (2001). When bad things turn good and good things turn bad: Sequences of redemption and contamination in life narrative and their relation to psychosocial adaptation in midlife adults and in students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 474–485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. McAdams, D. P. (2008). Personal narratives and the life story. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd edn.). New York: Guilford Press, pp. 242–262.Google Scholar
  27. McGregor, I., & Holmes, J. G. (1999). How storytelling shapes memory and impressions of relationship events over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(3), 403–419. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.76.3.403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. McLean, K. C., Pasupathi, M., & Pals, J. L. (2007). Selves creating stories creating selves: A process model of self-development. Personality and Social Psychology Review: An Official Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, 11(3), 262–278. doi: 10.1177/1088868307301034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. McLean, K. C., & Pratt, M. W. (2006). Life’s little (and big) lessons: Identity statuses and meaning-making in the turning point narratives of emerging adults. Developmental Psychology, 42(4), 714–722. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.714.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111(2), 486–511. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.2.486.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Niederhoffer, K. G., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2002). Linguistic style matching in social interaction. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 21(4), 337–360. doi: 10.1177/026192702237953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Nils, F., & Rimé, B. (2012). Beyond the myth of venting: Social sharing modes determine benefits from emotional disclosure. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 672–681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Pascuzzi, D., & Smorti, A. (2016). Emotion regulation, autobiographical memories and life narratives. New Ideas in Psychology, 45(1), 28–37.Google Scholar
  34. Pasupathi, M. (2001). The social construction of the personal past and its implications for adult development. Psychological Bulletin, 127(5), 651–672.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Pasupathi, M. (2003). Emotion regulation during social remembering: Differences between emotions elicited during an event and emotions elicited when talking about it. Memory (Hove, England), 11(2), 151–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Pasupathi, M., Alderman, K., & Shaw, D. (2007). Talking the talk: Collaborative remembering and self-perceived expertise. Discourse Processes, 43(1), 55–77. doi: 10.1080/01638530709336893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Pasupathi, M., & Hoyt, T. (2009). The development of narrative identity in late adolescence and emergent adulthood: The continued importance of listeners. Developmental Psychology, 45, 558–574.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Pasupathi, M., & Oldroyd, K. (2015). Telling and remembering: Complexities in long-term effects of listeners on autobiographical memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29(6), 835–842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Pasupathi, M., Stallworth, L. M., & Murdoch, K. (1998). How what we tell becomes what we know: Listener effects on speakers’ long-term memory for events. Discourse Processes, 26, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Pennebaker, J. W., & Seagal, J. D. (1999). Forming a story: The health benefits of narrative. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55(10), 1243–1254. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199910)55:10<1243::AID-JCLP6>3.0.CO;2-N.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Peterson, C., Bonechi, A., Smorti, A., & Tani, F. (2010). A distant mirror: Memories of parents and friends across childhood and adolescence. British Journal of Psychology (London, England: 1953), 101(4), 601–620. doi: 10.1348/000712609X478835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Pillemer, D. B. (1998). What is remembered about early childhood events? Clinical Psychology Review, 18(8), 895–913.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Rimé, B. (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: Theory and empirical review. Emotion Review, 1, 60–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Rubin, D. C., Rahhal, T. A., & Poon, L. W. (1998). Things learned in early adulthood are remembered best. Memory & Cognition, 26(1), 3–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Sales, J. M., Fivush, R., & Peterson, C. (2003). Parental reminiscing about positive and negative events. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4(2), 185–209. doi: 10.1207/S15327647JCD0402_03.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Shulman, S. (1993). Close relationships and coping behavior in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 16, 267–283.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Singer, J. A. (2004). Narrative identity and meaning making across the adult lifespan: An introduction. Journal of Personality, 72(3), 437–460.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1993). The remembered self: Emotion and memory in personality. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  49. Skowronski, J. J., & Walker, W. R. (2004). How describing autobiographical events can affect autobiographical memories. Social Cognition, 22(5), 555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Smorti, A., & Fioretti, C. (2016). Why narrating changes memory: A contribution to an integrative model of memory and narrative processes. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 50, 296–319. doi: 10.1007/s12124-015-9330-6.
  51. Smyth, J. M., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2008). Exploring the boundary conditions of expressive writing: In search of the right recipe. British Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 1–7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Thoman, D. B., Sansone, C., & Pasupathi, M. (2006). Talking about interest: Exploring the role of social interaction for regulating motivation and the interest experience. Journal of Happiness Studies, 8(3), 335–370. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9016-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Tulving, E., & Craik, F. I. (2000). The Oxford handbook of memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Tversky, B., & Marsh, E. J. (2000). Biased retellings of events yield biased memories. Cognitive Psychology, 40, 1–38.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Vygotsky, L. S. (1965, En.Ed). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  57. Zimmer-Gembeck, M. (2002). The development of romantic relationships and adaptations in the system of peer relationships. Journal of Adolescent Health, 31, 216–225.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Zimmermann, P., & Iwanski, A. (2014). Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood age differences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental variations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(2), 182–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Chiara Fioretti
    • 1
  • Debora Pascuzzi
    • 1
  • Andrea Smorti
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Educational Sciences and PsychologyUniversity of FlorenceFirenzeItaly

Personalised recommendations