Journal of Adult Development

, Volume 19, Issue 3, pp 150–157 | Cite as

The Impact of Processing Speed Training on Working Memory in Old Adults

Article

Abstract

Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have gotten different results as to whether processing speed can explain the aging of cognitive functions. Experimental analyses are needed to develop further evidence. To explore the relationship between speed and working memory in cognitive aging, processing speed intervention is conducted in the present study. Utilizing paper–pencil training, computer training, and a control group, 78 adults aged 58–83 years from Beijing participated in a speed intervention study. After 5 weeks of training, there were substantial training gains on two speed tests in two training groups, but no transfer effect on working memory was found. Discussion: The results fail to support the view that processing speed is a foundation of working memory for aging. The aging of working memory may depend more on an executive attention component.

Keywords

Processing speed Working memory Training Cognitive aging 

References

  1. Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ball, K., Berch, D. B., Helmers, K. F., Jobe, J. B., Leveck, M. D., Marsiske, M., et al. (2002). Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 13, 2271–2281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ball, K., Edwards, J. D., & Ross, L. A. (2007). The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday functions. The Journals of Gerontology, 62B, 19–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bell, R., Buchner, A., & Mund, I. (2008). Age-related differences in irrelevant-speech effects. Psychology and Aging, 23(2), 377–391.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Birren, J. E. (1974). Translations in gerontology: From lab to life. Psychophysiology and speed of response. American Psychologist, 29, 808–815.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bryan, J., & Luszcz, M. A. (1996). Speed of information processing as a mediator between age and free-recall performance. Psychology and Aging, 11(1), 3–9.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Deary, I. J. (2000). Looking down on human intelligence. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Deary, I. J., Allerhand, M., & Der, M. (2009). Smarter in middle age, faster in old age: A cross-lagged panel analysis of reaction time and cognitive ability over 13 years in the west of Scotland Twenty-07 Study. Psychology and Aging, 24, 40–47.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Finkel, D., Reynolds, C. A., McArdle, J. J., & Pedersen, N. L. (2005). The longitudinal relationship between processing speed and cognitive ability: Genetic and environmental influences. Behavior Genetics, 35, 535–549.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Finkel, D., Reynolds, C. A., McArdle, J. J., & Pedersen, N. L. (2007). Age changes in processing speed as a leading indicator of cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging, 22, 558–568.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fry, A. F., & Hale, S. (1996). Processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence: Evidence for a developmental cascade. Psychological Science, 7, 237–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Hershey, D. A., Boyd, M. L., Coutant, K. M., & Turner, K. (1999). Cognitive aging psychology: Significant advances, challenges, and training issues. Educational Gerontology, 25, 349–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hitch, G. J., & Towse, J. N. (1995). Working memory: What develops? In F. E. Weinert & W. Schneider (Eds.), Memory performance and competencies: Issues in growth and development (pp. 3–21). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  15. Hitch, G., Towse, J. N., & Hutton, U. (2001). What limits children’s working memory span? Theoretical accounts and applications for scholastic development. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(2), 184–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 637–671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(1), 47–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Laurence, B. L., Weismer, S. W., Miller, C. A., Francis, D. J., et al. (2007). Speed of processing, working memory, and language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(2), 408–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Li, D., Liu, C., Chen, T., & Li, G. (2004). The role of processing speed and working memory in life span cognitive development. Journal of Nanjing Normal University (Social Science), 1, 81–87.Google Scholar
  20. Madden, D. J. (2001). Speed and timing of behavioral processes. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (5th ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  21. Meijer, W. A., de Groot, R. H. M., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Van Gerven, P. W. M., & Jolles, J. (2006). Verbal learning and aging: Combined effects of irrelevant speech, inerstimulus interval and education. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 25(1), 364–376.Google Scholar
  22. Park, D. C. (2000). The basic mechanisms accounting for age related decline in cognitive function. In: Cognitive aging. New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  23. Park, D. C., Smith, A. D., Lautenschlager, G., Earles, J. L., Frieske, D., Zwahr, M., et al. (1996). Mediators of long-term memory performance across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 11, 621–637.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Peng, H., Shen, J., & Wang, D. (2004). The roles of working memory capacity and processing speed in inductive reasoning aging. Psychological Science, 27, 536–539.Google Scholar
  25. Peng, H., Shen, J., & Wang, D. (2006). Interrelations of visual function, processing speed and working memory in cognitive aging. Chinese Journal of Gerontology, 26(1), 1–3.Google Scholar
  26. Salthouse, T. A. (1985). Speed of behavior and its implications for cognition. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (2nd ed., pp. 400–426). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
  27. Salthouse, T. A. (1990). Cognitive competence and expertise in aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (3rd ed., pp. 310–319). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  28. Salthouse, T. A. (1993). Speed mediation of adult age differences in cognition. Developmental Psychology, 29, 722–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Salthouse, T. A. (1995). Aging, inhibition, working memory, and speed. The Journals of Gerontology, 50, 297–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Salthouse, T. A. (2001). Structural models of the relations between age and measures of cognitive functioning. Intelligence, 29, 93–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Salthouse, T. A. (2004). Localizing age-related individual differences in a hierarchical structure. Intelligence, 32, 541–561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Salthouse, T. A., & Ferrer-Caja, E. (2003). What needs to be explained to account for age-related effects on multiple cognitive variables? Psychology and Aging, 18, 91–110.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Salthouse, T. A., Fristoe, N., McGuthry, K. E., & Hambrick, D. Z. (1998). Relation of task switching to speed, age, and fluid intelligence. Psychology and Aging, 13(3), 445–461.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Schaie, K. W. (1989). Perceptual speed in adulthood: Cross-section and longitudinal studies. Psychology and Aging, 4, 443–453.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Schaie, K. W. (1994). Intellectual development in adulthood: The Seattle Longitudinal study. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Shen, J. L., Wang, D., Peng, H. M., & Tang, D. (2003). The effects of mediators on the aging of primary mental abilities. Acta Psychological Sinica, 6, 802–809.Google Scholar
  38. Vance, D., Dawson, J., Wadley, V., Edwards, J., Roenker, D., Rizzo, M., et al. (2007). The accelerate study: The longitudinal effect of speed of processing training on cognitive performance of older adults. Rehabilitation Psychology, 52, 89–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Verhaeghen, P., & Salthouse, T. A. (1997). Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood: Estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 122(3), 231–249.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Zimprich, D. (2002). Cross-sectionally and longitudinally balanced effects of processing speed on intellectual abilities. Experimental Aging Research, 28, 231–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Zimprich, D., & Martin, M. (2002). Can longitudinal changes in processing speed explain longitudinal age changes in fluid intelligence? Psychology and Aging, 17, 690–695.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute of Developmental PsychologyBeijing Normal UniversityBeijingChina

Personalised recommendations