Advertisement

Social Decision Making in Autistic Adolescents: The Role of Theory of Mind, Executive Functioning and Emotion Regulation

  • Kate Anne WoodcockEmail author
  • Catherine Cheung
  • Daniel González Marx
  • Will Mandy
Original Paper

Abstract

Social decision making is often challenging for autistic individuals. Twenty autistic adolescents made decisions in the socially interactive context of a one-shot ultimatum game, and performance was compared to a large matched typical reference sample. Theory of mind, executive functioning and emotion regulation were measured via direct assessments, self- and parent report. Relative to the reference sample, autistic adolescents proposed fewer fair offers, and this was associated with poorer theory of mind. Autistic adolescents responded similarly to the reference sample when making decisions about offers proposed to them, however they did not appear to down regulate their negative emotion in response to unfair treatment in the same way. Atypical processes may underpin even apparently typical decisions made by autistic adolescents.

Keywords

Autism spectrum disorder ASD Social decision making Emotion regulation Executive functioning Theory of mind 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the valuable contributions of the research participants, and the schools in the north of Ireland that supported the work. We also acknowledge the contribution of Emma Catterson, Eavan Hennessey and Ruth Shields in supporting the collection of data from the typical comparison group. The modified paradigm was piloted as a part of a Marie Curie Fellowship awarded to KW under the European Commission’s seventh framework programme (PIOF-GA-2009-252877), and supported with work by Raj Seraya Bhatoa and Eleanor Callaghan.

Author Contributions

KAW, CC and WM conceived and designed the study. All authors contributed to analysis planning, DGM conducted the analyses. All authors contributed to drafting the manuscript.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

Kate Anne Woodcock, Catherine Chung, Daniel González Marx and Will Mandy declares that they no conflicts of interest.

Ethics Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Supplementary material

10803_2019_3975_MOESM1_ESM.docx (2.1 mb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 2143 KB)

References

  1. Adams, M. P. (2013). Explaining the theory of mind deficit in autism spectrum disorder. Philosophical Studies, 163(1), 233–249.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098-011-9809-z.Google Scholar
  2. Ahlstrom, B. H., & Wentz, E. (2014). Difficulties in everyday life: Young persons with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorders perspectives. A chat-log analysis. International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-Being, 9, 10.  https://doi.org/10.3402/qhw.v9.23376.Google Scholar
  3. Baribeau, D. A., Doyle-Thomas, K. A., Dupuis, A., Iaboni, A., Crosbie, J., McGinn, H., ... Anagnostou, E. (2015). Examining and comparing social perception abilities across childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(6), 479–486.e471.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.03.016.Google Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 241–251.Google Scholar
  5. Berking, M., & Lukas, C. A. (2015). The Affect Regulation Training (ART): A transdiagnostic approach to the prevention and treatment of mental disorders. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 64–69.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.02.002.Google Scholar
  6. Bhanji, J. P., & Delgado, M. R. (2014). The social brain and reward: Social information processing in the human striatum. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5(1), 61–73.  https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1266.Google Scholar
  7. Blakemore, S. J. (2012). Imaging brain development: The adolescent brain. Neuroimage, 61(2), 397–406.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.080.Google Scholar
  8. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion—The self-assessment mannequin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9.Google Scholar
  9. Buescher, A. S., Cidav, Z., Knapp, M., & Mandell, D. (2014). Costs of autism spectrum disorders in the United Kingdom and the United States. JAMA Pediatrics, 168(8), 721–728.  https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.210.Google Scholar
  10. Buon, M., Dupoux, E., Jacob, P., Chaste, P., Leboyer, M., & Zalla, T. (2012). The role of causal and intentional judgments in moral reasoning in individuals with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(2), 458–470.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1588-7.Google Scholar
  11. Burns, A., Irvine, M., & Woodcock, K. A. (2019). Self-focused attention and depressive symptoms in adults with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(2), 692–703.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3732-5.Google Scholar
  12. Camerer, C., & Thaler, R. H. (1995). Anomalies: Ultimatums, dictators and manners. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 9(2), 209–219.Google Scholar
  13. Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., & Hare, T. A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 111–126.Google Scholar
  14. Channon, S., Fitzpatrick, S., Drury, H., Taylor, I., & Lagnado, D. (2010). Punishment and sympathy judgments: Is the quality of Mercy strained in Asperger’s syndrome? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(10), 1219–1226.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0980-4.Google Scholar
  15. Channon, S., Lagnado, D., Fitzpatrick, S., Drury, H., & Taylor, I. (2011). Judgments of cause and blame: Sensitivity to intentionality in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(11), 1534–1542.Google Scholar
  16. Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 231–239.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007.Google Scholar
  17. Chiu, P. H., Kayali, M. A., Kishida, K. T., Tomlin, D., Klinger, L. G., Klinger, M. R., & Montague, P. R. (2008). Self responses along cingulate cortex reveal quantitative neural phenotype for high-functioning autism. Neuron, 57(3), 463–473.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.020.Google Scholar
  18. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1987). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioural science. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  19. Cornish, K., Scerif, G., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Tracing syndrome-specific trajectories of attention across the lifespan. Cortex, 43(6), 672–685.  https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70497-0.Google Scholar
  20. Corradi-Dell’Acqua, C., Koban, L., Leiberg, S., & Vuilleumier, P. (2016). Editorial: What determines social behavior? Investigating the role of emotions, self-centered motives, and social norms. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 5.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00342.Google Scholar
  21. Craig, F., Margari, F., Legrottaglie, A. R., Palumbi, R., de Giambattista, C., & Margari, L. (2016). A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 12, 1191–1202.  https://doi.org/10.2147/ndt.s104620.Google Scholar
  22. Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., Kenworthy, L., & Baron, I. S. (2000). Test review: Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology, 6(3), 235–238.  https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.6.3.235.3152.Google Scholar
  23. Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Retzlaff, P. D., & Espy, K. A. (2002). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in a clinical sample. Child Neuropsychology, 8(4), 249–257.  https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.8.4.249.13513.Google Scholar
  24. Harlé, K. M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2007). Incidental sadness biases social economic decisions in the ultimatum game. Emotion, 7(4), 876–881.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.876.Google Scholar
  25. de Martino, B., Harrison, N. A., Knafo, S., Bird, G., & Dolan, R. J. (2008). Explaining enhanced logical consistency during decision making in autism. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(42), 10746–10750.  https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2895-08.2008.Google Scholar
  26. de Rosnay, M., Fink, E., Begeer, S., Slaughter, V., & Peterson, C. (2014). Talking theory of mind talk: Young school-aged children’s everyday conversation and understanding of mind and emotion. Journal of Child Language, 41, 1179–1193.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000913000433.Google Scholar
  27. de Veld, D. M. J., Howlin, P., Hoddenbach, E., Mulder, F., Wolf, I., Koot, H. M., ... Begeer, S. (2017). Moderating effects of parental characteristics on the effectiveness of a theory of mind training for children with autism: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(7), 1987–1997.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3117-1.Google Scholar
  28. Downs, A., & Smith, T. (2004). Emotional understanding, cooperation, and social behavior in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(6), 625–635.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-004-5284-0.Google Scholar
  29. Edmiston, E. K., Merkle, K., & Corbett, B. A. (2015). Neural and cortisol responses during play with human and computer partners in children with autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(8), 1074–1083.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu159.Google Scholar
  30. Ewing, L., Caulfield, F., Read, A., & Rhodes, G. (2015). Appearance-based trust behaviour is reduced in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 19(8), 1002–1009.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361314559431.Google Scholar
  31. Farley, M. A., McMahon, W. M., Fombonne, E., Jenson, W. R., Miller, J., Gardner, M., … Coon, H. (2009). Twenty-year outcome for individuals with autism and average or near-average cognitive abilities. Autism Research, 2(2), 109–118.  https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.69.Google Scholar
  32. Funahashi, S. (2017). Prefrontal contribution to decision-making under free-choice conditions. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 11, 15.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2017.00431.Google Scholar
  33. Gleichgerrcht, E., Torralva, T., Rattazzi, A., Marenco, V., Roca, M., & Manes, F. (2013). Selective impairment of cognitive empathy for moral judgment in adults with high functioning autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(7), 780–788.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss067.Google Scholar
  34. Grondhuis, S. N., Lecavalier, L., Arnold, L. E., Handen, B. L., Scahill, L., McDougle, C. J., & Aman, M. G. (2018). Differences in verbal and nonverbal IQ test scores in children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 49, 47–55.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2018.02.001.Google Scholar
  35. Güth, W., Schmittberger, R., & Schwarze, B. (1982). An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 3(4), 367–388.Google Scholar
  36. Halali, E., Bereby-Meyer, Y., & Ockenfels, A. (2013). Is it all about the self? The effect of self-control depletion on ultimatum game proposers. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–8.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00240.Google Scholar
  37. Howlin, P. (2000). Outcome in adult life for more able individuals with autism or Asperger syndrome. Autism, 4(1), 63–83.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361300004001005.Google Scholar
  38. Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Adult outcome for children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 212–229.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00215.x.Google Scholar
  39. Hull, L., Petrides, K. V., Allison, C., Smith, P., Baron-Cohen, S., Lai, M.-C., & Mandy, W. (2017). “Putting on my best normal”: Social camouflaging in adults with autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(8), 2519–2534.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5.Google Scholar
  40. Karbach, J., & Unger, K. (2014). Executive control training from middle childhood to adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 14.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00390.Google Scholar
  41. Kenny, L., Hattersley, C., Molins, B., Buckley, C., Povey, C., & Pellicano, E. (2016). Which terms should be used to describe autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism, 20(4), 442–462.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315588200.Google Scholar
  42. Kishida, K. T., King-Casas, B., & Montague, P. R. (2010). Neuroeconomic approaches to mental disorders. Neuron, 67(4), 543–554.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.07.021.Google Scholar
  43. Klapwijk, E. T., Aghajani, M., Lelieveld, G. J., van Lang, N. D. J., Popma, A., van der Wee, N. J. A., ... Vermeiren, R. (2017). Differential fairness decisions and brain responses after expressed emotions of others in boys with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(8), 2390–2400.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3159-4.Google Scholar
  44. Klin, A., Saulnier, C. A., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., Volkmar, F. R., & Lord, C. (2006). Social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: The Vineland and the ADOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 748–759.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0229-4.Google Scholar
  45. Knapp, M., Romeo, R., & Beecham, J. (2009). Economic cost of autism in the UK. Autism, 13(3), 317–336.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361309104246.Google Scholar
  46. Li, J., Zhu, L. Q., & Gummerum, M. (2014). The relationship between moral judgment and cooperation in children with high-functioning autism. Scientific Reports, 4, 6.  https://doi.org/10.1038/srep04314.Google Scholar
  47. Loomes, R., Hull, L., & Mandy, W. P. L. (2017). What is the male-to-female ratio in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(6), 466–474.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013.Google Scholar
  48. Mazefsky, C. A., Herrington, J., Siegel, M., Scarpa, A., Maddox, B. B., Scahill, L., & White, S. W. (2013). The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(7), 679–688.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.006.Google Scholar
  49. Mazefsky, C. A., Pelphrey, K. A., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). The need for a broader approach to emotion regulation research in autism. Child Development Perspectives, 6(1), 92–97.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00229.x.Google Scholar
  50. Merikangas, K. R., He, J., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., et al. (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in US adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(10), 980–989.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017.Google Scholar
  51. Milosavljevic, B., Leno, V. C., Simonoff, E., Baird, G., Pickles, A., Jones, C. R. G., ... Happe, F. (2016). Alexithymia in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: Its relationship to internalising difficulties, sensory modulation and social cognition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(4), 1354–1367.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2670-8.Google Scholar
  52. Moran, J. M., Young, L. L., Saxe, R., Lee, S. M., O’Young, D., Mavros, P. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2011). Impaired theory of mind for moral judgment in high-functioning autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2688–2692.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1011734108.Google Scholar
  53. Morsanyi, K., Handley, S. J., & Evans, J. S. B. T. (2010). Decontextualised minds: Adolescents with autism are less susceptible to the conjunction fallacy than typically developing adolescents. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(11), 1378–1388.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0993-z.Google Scholar
  54. Pantelis, P. C., & Kennedy, D. P. (2017). Autism does not limit strategic thinking in the “beauty contest” game. Cognition, 160, 91–97.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.12.015.Google Scholar
  55. Patel, S., Day, T. N., Jones, N., & Mazefsky, C. A. (2017). Association between anger rumination and autism symptom severity, depression symptoms, aggression, and general dysregulation in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 21(2), 181–189.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316633566.Google Scholar
  56. Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy - Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1–2), 8–13.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017.Google Scholar
  57. Pellicano, E., Dinsmore, A., & Charman, T. (2014). What should autism research focus upon? Community views and priorities from the United Kingdom. Autism, 18(7), 756–770.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361314529627.Google Scholar
  58. Peterson, C., Slaughter, V., Moore, C., & Wellman, H. M. (2015). Peer social skills and theory of mind development in children with autism, deafness, or typical development. Developmental Psychology, 25, 46–57.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039833.Google Scholar
  59. Pillutla, M. M., & Murnighan, J. K. (1996). Unfairness, anger, and spite: Emotional rejections of ultimatum offers. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68(3), 208–224.  https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1996.0100.Google Scholar
  60. Richey, J. A., Damiano, C. R., Sabatino, A., Rittenberg, A., Petty, C., Bizzell, J., ... Dichter, G. S. (2015). Neural mechanisms of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(11), 3409–3423.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2359-z.Google Scholar
  61. Robb, N., Waller, A., & Woodcock, K. A. (2015). The Development of TASTER, a cognitive training game using human-centered design, tailored for children with global and specific cognitive impairments. In IEEE 7th International Conference on Games and Virtual Worlds for Serious Applications (Vs-Games), 208–209.Google Scholar
  62. Robb, N., Waller, A. & Woodcock, K. A. (in press). Developing a task switching training game for children with a rare genetic syndrome linked to intellectual disability. Simulation and Gaming.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1046878119834319.
  63. Rowley, S. J., & Camacho, T. C. (2015). Increasing diversity in cognitive developmental research: Issues and solutions. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(5), 683–692.  https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2014.976224.Google Scholar
  64. Sally, D., & Hill, E. (2006). The development of interpersonal strategy: Autism, theory-of-mind, cooperation and fairness. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(1), 73–97.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2005.06.015.Google Scholar
  65. Samson, A. C., Hardan, A. Y., Podell, R. W., Phillips, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2015a). Emotion regulation in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 8(1), 9–18.  https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1387.Google Scholar
  66. Samson, A. C., Wells, W. M., Phillips, J. M., Hardan, A. Y., & Gross, J. J. (2015b). Emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from parent interviews and children’s daily diaries. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(8), 903–913.  https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12370.Google Scholar
  67. Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300(5626), 1755–1758.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1082976.Google Scholar
  68. Sawilowsky, S. S. (2009). New effect size rules of thumb. Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods, 8(2), 597–599. Available at: http://digitalcommons.wayne.edu/coe_tbf/4.
  69. Schmitz, E. A., Banerjee, R., Pouw, L. B. C., Stockmann, L., & Rieffe, C. (2015). Better to be equal? Challenges to equality for cognitively able children with autism spectrum disorders in a social decision game. Autism, 19(2), 178–186.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361313516547.Google Scholar
  70. Schoorl, J., van Rijn, S., de Wied, M., van Goozen, S., & Swaab, H. (2016). Emotion regulation difficulties in boys with oppositional defiant disorder/conduct disorder and the relation with comorbid autism traits and attention deficit traits. PLoS ONE, 11(7), 12.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0159323.Google Scholar
  71. Shulman, C., Guberman, A., Shiling, N., & Bauminger, N. (2011). Moral and social reasoning in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(7), 1364–1376.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1369-8.Google Scholar
  72. Tabibnia, G., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). The sunny side of fairness—Preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry). Psychological Science, 19(4), 339–347.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02091.x.Google Scholar
  73. Tayama, M., Tateno, M., Park, T. W., Ukai, W., Hashimoto, E., & Saito, T. (2012). The study of cognitive characteristics in Asperger’s disorder by using a modified Prisoner’s Dilemma game with a variable payoff matrix. PLoS ONE, 7(11), 7.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048794.Google Scholar
  74. Tomasello, M., & Vaish, A. (2013). Origins of human cooperation and morality. In S. T. Fiske (Ed.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 64, pp. 231–255). Palo Alto: Annual Reviews.Google Scholar
  75. Tremblay, S., Sharika, K. M., & Platt, M. L. (2017). Social decision-making and the brain: A comparative perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(4), 265–276.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.007.Google Scholar
  76. van Bergeijk, E., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2008). Supporting more able students on the autism spectrum: College and beyond. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(7), 1359–1370.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0524-8.Google Scholar
  77. van der Meulen, A., Roerig, S., de Ruyter, D., van Lier, P., & Krabbendam, L. (2017). A comparison of children’s ability to read children’s and adults’ mental states in an adaptation of the reading the mind in the eyes task. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(594), 594.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00594.Google Scholar
  78. van Heijst, B. F., & Geurts, H. M. (2015). Quality of life in autism across the lifespan: A meta-analysis. Autism, 19(2), 158–167.Google Scholar
  79. van’t Wout, M., Chang, L. J., & Sanfey, A. G. (2010). The influence of emotion regulation on social interactive decision-making. Emotion, 10(6), 815–821.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020069.Google Scholar
  80. Wang, C. S., Sivanathan, N., Narayanan, J., Ganegoda, D. B., Bauer, M., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Murnighan, K. (2011). Retribution and emotional regulation: The effects of time delay in angry economic interactions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 116(1), 46–54.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.05.007.Google Scholar
  81. Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  82. White, S., Hill, E., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2009). Revisiting the strange stories: Revealing mentalizing impairments in autism. Child development, 80(4), 1097–1117.Google Scholar
  83. Yoshida, W., Dziobek, I., Kliemann, D., Heekeren, H. R., Friston, K. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2010). Cooperation and heterogeneity of the Autistic mind. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(26), 8815–8818.  https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0400-10.2010.Google Scholar
  84. Zalla, T., & Leboyer, M. (2011). Judgment of intentionality and moral evaluation in individuals with high functioning autism. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2(4), 681–698.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-011-0048-1.Google Scholar
  85. Zantinge, G., van Rijn, S., Stockmann, L., & Swaab, H. (2017). Physiological arousal and emotion regulation strategies in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(9), 2648–2657.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3181-6.Google Scholar
  86. Zeman, J., Shipman, K., & Penza-Clyve, S. (2001). Development and initial validation of the children’s sadness management scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25(3), 187–205.  https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1010623226626.Google Scholar
  87. Zeman, J., Shipman, K., & Suveg, C. (2002). Anger and sadness regulation: Predictions to internalizing and externalizing symptoms in children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31(3), 393–398.  https://doi.org/10.1207/153744202760082658.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centre for Applied Psychology & Institute for Mental HealthUniversity of BirminghamBirminghamUK
  2. 2.Division of Psychology and Language SciencesUniversity College LondonLondonUK
  3. 3.School of PsychologyQueen’s University BelfastBelfastUK
  4. 4.School of PsychologyUniversity of BirminghamBirminghamUK
  5. 5.Faculty of PhysicsUniversity of GöttingenGöttingenGermany

Personalised recommendations