Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 49, Issue 4, pp 1559–1569 | Cite as

Autism and Religious Beliefs: Clues from Kafka Research

  • Jerry StugerEmail author
Original Paper

Abstract

Religious beliefs and faith in high-functioning autistic persons are analyzed by discussing the work and biography of Franz Kafka. It enables researchers to differentiate between how autistic and non-autistic persons experience and express religious beliefs and spiritual issues because Kafka as an autistic person also wrote about these topics in his philosophical–religious writings. This Franz Kafka case study provides additional inner (cognitive) state input to better understand high-functioning autism. This contribution with new and different perspectives to the field of autism and religious beliefs could help to explore the varieties of religious experience and cognition, and to improve our insights in high-functioning autism.

Keywords

Franz Kafka Kierkegaard Autism and religion The self Self-awareness Kafkaesque Autistic cognitive style Asperger syndrome High-functioning autism Perspective-taking 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

I wish to confirm that there are no known conflict of interest associated with this publication and there has been no financial support for this work that could have influenced its outcome.

Ethical Approval

This article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals performed by any of the authors.

References

  1. Apperly, I. A. (2008). Beyond simulation theory and theory–theory: Why social cognitive neuroscience should use its own concepts to study “Theory of Mind”. Cognition, 107(1), 266–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Badcock, C. (2004). Mentalism and Mechanism. The twin modes of human cognition. In Crawford, C. & Salmon, C. (Eds.), Evolutionary psychology, public policy and personal decisions, (pp. 99–116). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). The cognitive neuroscience of autism: Evolutionary approaches. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (2nd edn., pp. 1249–1257). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001a). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Spong, A., Scahill, V., & Lawson, J. (2001b). Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 5, 47–78.Google Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Stone, V., & Rutherford, M. (1999). A mathematician, a physicist, and a computer scientist with Asperger syndrome: Performance on folk psychology and folk physics tests. Neurocase, 5, 475–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bering, J. M. (2002). The existential theory of mind. Review of General Psychology, 6(1), 3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Binnie, L., & Williams, J. (2003). Intuitive psychology and physics among children with autism and typically developing children. Autism, 7(2), 173–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brezis, R. S. (2012). Autism as a case for neuroanthropology: Delineating the role of theory of mind in religious development. In D. H. Lende & G. Downey (Eds.), The encultured brain An introduction to neuroanthropology (pp. 291–314). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  10. Caldwell-Harris, C., Murphy, C. F., & Velazquez, T. (2011). Religious belief systems of persons with high functioning autism. In L. Carlson, C. Hoelscher, & T. F. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 3362–3366). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
  11. Chamak, B., et al. (2008). What can we learn about Autism from autistic persons? Psychother Psychosom, 77, 271–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Damasio, A. (1994). The Neural Self. In Descartes’ error. Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain (pp. 236–244). New York: Avon Books.Google Scholar
  13. Damasio, A. (2010). Self comes to mind. Constructing the conscious brain. New York: Pantheon Books, a division of Random House, Inc.Google Scholar
  14. Darrow, R. A. (2005). Kierkegaard, Kafka, and the strength of “The Absurd” in Abraham’s sacrifice of Isaac. Dissertation for the degree of Master of Humanities. Wright State University.Google Scholar
  15. David, C. (1980). ‘Die Geschichte Abrahams: Zu Kafkas Auseinanderung mit Kierkegaard. ‘Bild und Gedanke: Festschrift für Gehart Baumann zum 60 Geburtsdag. Ed. G. Schnitzler et al. München: Wilhelm Fink Verlag, pp. 79–90.Google Scholar
  16. Davidson, J., & Smith, M. (2009). Autistic autobiographies and more than human emotional geographies. Environment and Planning D, 27, 898–916.  https://doi.org/10.1068/d4308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Deeley, Q. (2009). Cognitive style, spirituality, and religious understanding: The case of autism. Journal of Religion Disability Health, 13, 77–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dennet, D. C. (1987). Chapter 2: True believers: The Intentional strategy and why it works. In The intentional stance. A bradford book (pp. 13–35). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  19. Ekblad, L., & Oviedo, L. (2017). Religious cognition among subjects with autism spectrum disorder (ASD): Defective or different? Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 14(4), 287–296.Google Scholar
  20. Elmose, M. (2016). A conceptual framework for understanding characteristics of self-awareness associated with autism spectrum disorder. Scandinavian Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychology, 4(3), 109–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Frith, U. (2012). Why we need cognitive explanations of autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(11), 2073–2092.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.697178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Goebel, R. J. (1985). Kafka and Kierkegaard’s fear and trembling: Critique and revision. Journal of the Kafka Society of America, 9 (1985), 69–85.Google Scholar
  23. Graetz, J. E., & Dubin, N. (2009). Through a different lens: Spirituality in the lives of Individuals with Asperger’s Syndrome. Journal of Religion Disability & Health, 13, 29–39.  https://doi.org/10.1080/15228960802505213. 2009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Heywood, B. T., & Bering, J. M. (2013).’Meant to Be’: How religious beliefs and cultural religiosity affect the implicit bias to think teleologically. Religion Brain & Behavior.  https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2013.782888.Google Scholar
  25. Huang, A. X., Hughes, T. L., Sutton, L. R., Lawrence, M., Chen, X., Ji, Z., & Zeleke, W. (2017). Understanding the self in individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD): A review of literature. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1422.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kafka, F. (1991). The blue octavo notebooks. In M. Brod (Ed.), Translated Ernst Kaiser and Eithne Wilkins (p. 105). Cambridge: Exact Change.Google Scholar
  27. Kalkman, D. P. (2014). Three cognitive routes to atheism: A dual-process account. Religion. 44(1), 72–83.  https://doi.org/10.1080/0048721X.2013.841110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Leslie, A. M. (1994). ToMM, ToBy, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. Mapping the mind. Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture, 11, 119–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lindeman, M., & Lipsanen, J. (2016). Diverse cognitive profiles of religious believers and Nonbelievers. The International Journal for The Psychology of Religion, 26(3), 1–8.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2015.1091695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lindeman, M., & Svedholm-Häkkinen, A. M. (2016). Does poor understanding of physical world predict religious and paranormal beliefs? Applied cognitive psychology.  https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3248.
  31. Lindeman, M., Svedholm-Häkkinen, A. M., & Lipsanen, J. (2015). Ontological confusions but not mentalizing abilities predict religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in supernatural purpose. Cognition, 134, 63–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lyons, V., & Fitzgerald, M. (2013). Atypical sense of self in autism spectrum disorders: A neuro-cognitive perspective. Recent Advances in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1, 749–770.Google Scholar
  33. Martin, J., Sugerman, J. H., & Hickinbottom, S. (2010). Chapter 9: Perspectives and Persons: Ontological constitutive possibilities. In: G. Persons (Ed.) Understanding psychological selfhood and agency (pp. 137–153). Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. McCauley, R. N. (2011). Why religion is natural and science is not. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  35. McFry, B. (2007). Finding and following the true way: Franz Kafka’s Zürau Aphorisms. Thesis for the degree of Master of Arts. Athens Georgia.Google Scholar
  36. Molnar-Szakacs, & Uddin, L. Q. (2016). Chapter 15: The Self in Autism. In M. Kyrios (Ed.) The self in understanding and treating psychological disorders (pp. 144–157). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Morin, A. (2006). Levels of consciousness and self-awareness: A comparison and integration of various neurocognitive views. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(2), 358–371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Morton, J. (2004). Understanding developmental disorders. A causal modelling approach. New York: Blackwell Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Morton, J., & Frith, U. (2001). Why we need cognition: Cause and Developmental disorder. In: E. Dupoux (Ed). Language, brain and cognitive development (pp. 263–278). New York: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  40. Müller, B. (2010). Consolation in your neighbor’s fur: On Kafka’s animal parables. In M. Lucht & D. Yarri (Eds.), Kafka’s creatures. Animals, hybrids and othe fantastic beings (p. 114). Lanham: Lexington Books.Google Scholar
  41. Norenzayan, A., & Gervais, W. M. (2013). The origins of religious disbelief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 20–25.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.11.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Norenzayan, A., Gervais, W. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2012). Mentalizing deficits constrain belief in a personal God. PLoS ONE, 7(5), e36880.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036880.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Paganini, C., & Gaido, D. (2013). Perception in autism: An interactive global research between folk psychology and folk physics. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 7(2), 175–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Powell, M. (2012). A tale of two abrahams: Kafka, Kierkegaard, and the possibility of faith in the modern world. The Heythrop Journal LIII, pp. 61–70.Google Scholar
  45. Reddish, P., Tok, P., & Kundt, R. (2015). Religious cognition and behaviour in autism: The role of mentalizing. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 26, 1–18.  https://doi.org/10.10810/10508619.2014.1003518.Google Scholar
  46. Schaap-Jonker, H., et al. (2013). Autism spectrum disorders and the image of god as a core aspect of religiousness. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 23, 145–160.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2012.688005. 2013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Slochower, H. (1946) The limitations of Franz Kafka. The American Scholar, 15 (pp. 291–297). Washington: The Phi Beta Kappa Society.Google Scholar
  48. Stach, R. (2013). Kafka—The Years of Insight. Translated from the German by Shelley Frisch (pp. 237–243). Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  49. Stuger, J. (2017). Kafka and Autism. The undisclosed logic behind Kafka’s work. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3158-5.Google Scholar
  50. Swinton, J., & Trevett, C. (2009). Religion and autism: Initiating an Interdisciplinary conversation. Journal of Religion Disability & Health, 13, 2–6.  https://doi.org/10.1080/15228960802606193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. (2005). How language facilitates the acquisition of false belief understanding in children with autism. In J. Astington & J. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters in theory of mind (pp. 298–318). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Tremlin, T. (2006). Minds and gods: The cognitive foundations of religion (p. 104). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Verhaar, H. (1975). Franz Kafka of Schrijven uit onmacht (pp. 89–93). Amsterdam. Wetenschappelijk Uitgeverij, BV.Google Scholar
  54. Visuri, I. (2012). Could Everyone talk to god? A case study on Asperger’s Syndrome, Religion, and Spirituality. Journal of Religion, Disability & Health, 16, 352–378.  https://doi.org/10.1080/15228967.2012.731888. 2012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Vonk, J., & Pitzen, J. (2017). Believing in other minds: Accurate mentalizing does not predict religiosity. Personality and Individual Differences, 115, 70–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Wahl, J. (1946). Kierkegaard and Kafka. Trans. Lienhard Burgel. The Kafka problem. Ed. Angel Flores. New York: Gordian. 1975. 277 – 90.Google Scholar
  57. Williams, D. (2010). Theory of own mind in autism. Evidence of a specific deficit in self-awareness. Autism International Journal of Research and Practice., 12(5), 474–494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Zahavi, D. (2010). Complexities of self. Autism, 14, 547–551.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361310370040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Independent ResearcherAmsterdamThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations