Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 47, Issue 9, pp 2743–2756 | Cite as

Simple Mindreading Abilities Predict Complex Theory of Mind: Developmental Delay in Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Maria Chiara Pino
  • Monica Mazza
  • Melania Mariano
  • Sara Peretti
  • Dagmara Dimitriou
  • Francesco Masedu
  • Marco Valenti
  • Fabia Franco
Original Paper

Abstract

Theory of mind (ToM) is impaired in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). The aims of this study were to: (i) examine the developmental trajectories of ToM abilities in two different mentalizing tasks in children with ASD compared to TD children; and (ii) to assess if a ToM simple test known as eyes-test could predict performance on the more advanced ToM task, i.e. comic strip test. Based on a sample of 37 children with ASD and 55 TD children, our results revealed slower development at varying rates in all ToM measures in children with ASD, with delayed onset compared to TD children. These results could stimulate new treatments for social abilities, which would lessen the social deficit in ASD.

Keywords

Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Developmental trajectories approach Eyes task Comic strip test (CST) Theory of mind (ToM) 

Notes

Author Contributions

MCP, MM, FF and MV designed research; MM and SP collected data; MCP, DD and FM analyzed the data; All authors have contributed to write the paper.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C. F., & Theakston, A. L. (2015). The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 42(2), 239–273.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-V (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Review Neuroscience, 7(4), 268–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Astington, J. W. (2001). The future of theory-of-mind research: Understanding motivational states, the role of language, and real-world consequences. Child Development, 72(3), 685–687CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 1311–1320.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30(2), 285–297.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen, S., Bowen, D. C., Holt, R. J., Allison, C., Auyeung, B., Lombardo, M. V., et al. (2015). The “reading the mind in the eyes” test: Complete absence of typical sex difference in ~400 men and women with autism. PLoS ONE 10(8), e0136521.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). The test for reception of grammar, version 2 (TROG-2). London: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  10. Broekhof, E., Ketelaar, L., Stockmann, L., van Zijp, A., Bos, M. G., & Rieffe, C. (2015). The understanding of intentions, desires and beliefs in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(7), 2035–2045.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Chapman, R. S., Hesketh, L. J., & Kistler, D. J. (2002). Predicting longitudinal change in language production and comprehension in individuals with down syndrome: Hierarchical linear modeling. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 45(5), 902–915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Charman, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1992). Understanding drawings and beliefs: A further test of the metarepresentation theory of autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(6), 1105–1112.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2013). Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 74, 195–203.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hill, J. (2007). Do children with autism have a theory of mind? A non-verbal test of autism vs. specific language impairment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 716–723.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Cornish, K., Rinehart, N., Gray, K., & Howlin, P. (2010). Comic strip task. Melbourne: Monash University Developmental Neuroscience and Genetic Disorders Laboratory and Monash University Centre for Developmental Psychiatry and Psychology.Google Scholar
  16. Denham, S. A., McKinley, M., Couchoud, E. A., & Holt, R. (1990). Emotional and behavioral predictors of preschool peer ratings. Child Development, 61(4), 1145–1152.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised (PPVT-R). Forms L and M. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  18. Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Pintilie, D. (1982). British picture vocabulary test. Windsor: NFER-NELASON.Google Scholar
  19. Eisenmajer, R., & Prior, M. (1991). Cognitive linguistic correlates of “theory of mind” ability in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 351–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Elman, J. L., Bates, E., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  21. Franco, F., Itakura, S., Pomorska, K., Abramowski, A., Nikaido, K., & Dimitriou, D. (2014). Can children with autism read emotions from the eyes? The eyes test revisited. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(5), 1015–1026.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2014). Annual research review: Towards a developmental neuroscience of atypical social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(6), 553–557.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Happe, F. G. (1994a). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Happe, F. G. (1994b). Wechsler IQ profile and theory of mind in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1461–1471.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Happe, F. G. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843–855.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Hasegawa, C., Kikuchi, M., Yoshimura, Y., Hiraishi, H., Munesue, T., Takesaki, N., et al. (2015). Changes in autistic trait indicators in parents and their children with ASD: A preliminary longitudinal study. Psychiatry Research 228(3), 956–957. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.05.048.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Hobson, R. P. (1984) Early childhood autism and the question of egocentrism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 85–104.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(10), 389–398.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Kover, S. T., Haebig, E., Oakes, A., McDuffie, A., Hagerman, R. J., & Abbeduto, L. (2014). Sentence comprehension in boys with autism spectrum disorder. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 23(3), 385–394.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Kumschick, I. R., Beck, L., Eid, M., Witte, G., Klann-Delius, G., Heuser, I., et al. (2014). READING and FEELING: The effects of a literature-based intervention designed to increase emotional competence in second and third graders. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1448.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2014). Autism. Lancet 383(9920):896–910.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Laws, G., & Bishop, D. (2003). A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(6):1324–1339.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Leekam, S., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a metarepresentational deficit? Cognition, 40, 203–218.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule, 2nd edition (ADOS-2) manual (Part I): Modules 1–4. Torrence, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  35. Mazza, M., Lucci, G., Pacitti, F., Pino, M. C., Mariano, M., Casacchia, M., et al. (2010). Could schizophrenic subjects improve their social cognition abilities only with observation and imitation of social situations? Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 20(5), 675–703.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Mazza, M., Pino, M. C., Mariano, M., Tempesta, D., Ferrara, M., De Berardis, D., et al (2014). Affective and cognitive empathy in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(8), 791.Google Scholar
  37. Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–646.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Nordberg, A., Dahlgren Sandberg, A., & Miniscalco, C. (2015). Story retelling and language ability in school-aged children with cerebral palsy and speech impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 50(6), 801–813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Oakes, A., Kover, S. T., & Abbeduto, L. (2013). Language comprehension profiles of young adolescents with fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 22(4), 615–626.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A. M., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Hill, M. M., Hutman, T., et al. (2010). A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(3), 256–266.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Ozonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudinal study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 415–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that …”: Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437–471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2005). Steps in theory-of-mind development for children with deafness or autism. Child Development, 76(2), 502–517.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Chimpanzee problem-solving: A test for comprehension. Science 202(4367), 532–535.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Prior, M. R., Dahlstrom, B., & Squires, T. L. (1990). Autistic children’s knowledge of thinking and feeling states in other people. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 587–601.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Sebastian, C. L., Fontaine, N. M., Bird, G., Blakemore, S. J., Brito, S. A., McCrory, E. J., et al. (2012). Neural processing associated with cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in adolescents and adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 53–63.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Harari, H., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Levkovitz, Y. (2010). The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in affective theory of mind deficits in criminal offenders with psychopathic tendencies. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 46(5), 668–677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Berger, B. D., Goldsher, D., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2005). Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cognitive and Behavioural Neurology, 18(1), 55–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Sivaratnam, C. S., Cornish, K., Gray, K. M., Howlin, P., & Rinehart, N. J. (2012). Brief report: Assessment of the social-emotional profile in children with autism spectrum disorders using a novel comic strip task. Journal of Autism and Development Disorders, 42(11), 2505–2512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Sparrevohn, R. & Howie, P. M. (1995). Theory of mind in children with autistic disorder: Evidence of developmental progression and the role of verbal ability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 249–263.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland adaptive behavior scales (2nd ed.). Vineland: Pearson.Google Scholar
  52. Thelen, E., & Corbetta, D. (1994). Exploration and selection in the early acquisition of skills. International Review of Neurobiology, 37, 75–102.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Thomas, M. S. C., Annaz, D., Ansari, D., Serif, G., Jarrold, C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Using developmental trajectories to understand developmental disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 336–358.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. van Geert, P. (1991). A dynamic systems model of cognitive and language growth. Psychological Review, 98, 3–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Vetter, N. C., Leipold, K., Kliegel, M., Phillips, L. H., & Altgassen, M. (2013). Ongoing development of social cognition in adolescence. Child Neuropsychology, 19(6), 615–629.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Yirmiya, N., Solomonica-Levi, D., Shulman, C., & Pilowsky, T. (1996). Theory of mind abilities in individuals with autism, down syndrome, and mental retardation of unknown etiology: The role of age and intelligence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(8), 1003–1014.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Yule, W. (1978). Research methodology: What are the “correct controls”?. In M. Rutter & E. Schopler (Eds.), Autism: A reappraisal of concepts and treatment (pp. 155–162). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Maria Chiara Pino
    • 1
  • Monica Mazza
    • 1
  • Melania Mariano
    • 1
  • Sara Peretti
    • 1
  • Dagmara Dimitriou
    • 2
  • Francesco Masedu
    • 4
  • Marco Valenti
    • 4
    • 5
  • Fabia Franco
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of Applied Clinical Sciences and BiotechnologyUniversity of L’AquilaL’aquilaItaly
  2. 2.Department of Psychology and Human DevelopmentUCL-Institute of EducationLondonUK
  3. 3.Department of Psychology, School of Science and TechnologyMiddlesex UniversityLondonUK
  4. 4.Department of Applied Clinical Sciences and Biotechnology, Section of Clinical Epidemiology and Environmental MedicineUniversity of L’AquilaL’aquilaItaly
  5. 5.Regional Centre for AutismAbruzzo Region Health SystemL’aquilaItaly

Personalised recommendations