Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 44, Issue 6, pp 1414–1424

Joint Attention Initiation With and Without Positive Affect: Risk Group Differences and Associations with ASD Symptoms

  • Devon N. Gangi
  • Lisa V. Ibañez
  • Daniel S. Messinger
Original Paper

Abstract

Infants at risk for autism spectrum disorders (ASD) may have difficulty integrating smiles into initiating joint attention (IJA) bids. A specific IJA pattern, anticipatory smiling, may communicate preexisting positive affect when an infant smiles at an object and then turns the smile toward the social partner. We compared the development of anticipatory smiling at 8, 10, and 12 months in infant siblings of children with ASD (high-risk siblings) and without ASD (low-risk siblings). High-risk siblings produced less anticipatory smiling than low-risk siblings, suggesting early differences in communicating preexisting positive affect. While early anticipatory smiling distinguished the risk groups, IJA not accompanied by smiling best predicted later severity of ASD-related behavioral characteristics among high-risk siblings. High-risk infants appear to show lower levels of motivation to share positive affect with others. However, facility with initiating joint attention in the absence of a clear index of positive affective motivation appears to be central to the prediction of ASD symptoms.

Keywords

Anticipatory smiling High-risk siblings Autism spectrum disorders Initiating joint attention 

References

  1. Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Deckner, D. F., & Romski, M. (2009). Joint engagement and the emergence of language in children with autism and Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 84–96. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0601-7.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4, text rev ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  3. Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interaction. Child Development, 55(4), 1278–1289. doi:10.2307/1129997.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bedford, R., Elsabbagh, M., Gliga, T., Pickles, A., Senju, A., Charman, T., et al. (2012). Precursors to social and communication difficulties in infants at-risk for autism: Gaze following and attentional engagement. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(10), 2208–2218. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1450-y.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Berument, S., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. (1999). Autism screening questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 444–451. doi:10.1192/bjp.175.5.444.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bloom, L., Beckwith, R., & Capatides, J. B. (1988). Developments in the expression of affect. Infant Behavior and Development, 11(2), 169–186. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(88)80004-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bloom, L., & Capatides, J. B. (1987). Expression of affect and the emergence of language. Child Development, 58(6), 1513–1522. doi:10.2307/1130691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cassel, T. D., Messinger, D. S., Ibanez, L. V., Haltigan, J. D., Acosta, S. I., & Buchman, A. C. (2007). Early social and emotional communication in the infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders: An examination of the broad phenotype. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 122–132. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0337-1.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Damiano, C. R., Nahmias, A., Hogan-Brown, A. L., & Stone, W. L. (2013). What do repetitive and stereotyped movements mean for infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(6), 1326–1335. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1681-y.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dawson, G., Hill, D., Spencer, A., Galpert, L., & Watson, L. (1990). Affective exchanges between young autistic children and their mothers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18(3), 335–345. doi:10.1007/BF00916569.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., et al. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–283. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.271.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial action coding system. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press.Google Scholar
  13. Georgiades, S., Szatmari, P., Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Brian, J., Roberts, W., et al. (2013). A prospective study of autistic-like traits in unaffected siblings of probands with autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(1), 42–48. doi:10.1001/2013.jamapsychiatry.1.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gerdts, J., & Bernier, R. (2011). The broader autism phenotype and its implications on the etiology and treatment of autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research and Treatment, 2011. doi: 10.1155/2011/545901.
  15. Goldberg, W. A., Jarvis, K. L., Osann, K., Laulhere, T. M., Straub, C., Thomas, E., et al. (2005). Brief report: Early social communication behaviors in the younger siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(5), 657–664. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0009-6.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gotham, K., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2009). Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(5), 693–705. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ibañez, L. V., Grantz, C. J., & Messinger, D. S. (2012). The development of referential communication and autism symptomatology in high-risk infants. Infancy,. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00142.x.Google Scholar
  18. Jones, S. S., Collins, K., & Hong, H. (1991). An audience effect on smile production in 10-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 2(1), 45–49. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00095.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Jones, S. S., & Hong, H. (2001). Onset of voluntary communication: Smiling looks to mother. Infancy, 2(3), 353–370. doi:10.1207/S15327078IN0203_4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jones, S. S., & Hong, H. (2005). How some infant smiles get made. Infant Behavior & Development, 28(2), 194–205. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.02.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Jones, S. S., & Raag, T. (1989). Smile production in older infants: The importance of a social recipient for the facial signal. Child Development, 60(4), 811–818. doi:10.2307/1131021.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1997). An investigation of attention and affect in children with autism and Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(4), 385–396. doi:10.1023/A:1025853321118.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. (1990). Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic, and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 87–100. doi:10.1007/BF02206859.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Landa, R., & Garrett-Mayer, E. (2006). Development in infants with autism spectrum disorders: A prospective study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(6), 629–638. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01531.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Messinger, D. S., & Fogel, A. (1998). Give and take: The development of conventional infant gestures. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 566–590.Google Scholar
  27. Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  28. Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Van Hecke, A., & Parlade, M. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development, 78(3), 938–954. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01042.x.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., & Siebert, J. (2003). A manual for the abridged Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS). Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
  30. Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 115–128. doi:10.1007/BF02206861.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Mundy, P., Sigman, M. D., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 27(5), 657–669. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1986.tb00190.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Mundy, P., & Vaughan, A. (2001). Joint attention and its role in the diagnostic assessment of children with autism. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 27(1–2), 57–60. doi:10.1177/073724770202700108.Google Scholar
  33. Nichols, C. M., Ibañez, L. V., Foss-Feig, J. H., & Stone, W. L. (2013). Social smiling and its components in high-risk infant siblings without later asd symptomatology. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1944-2.Google Scholar
  34. Ozonoff, S., Losif, A., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Hill, M., Hutman, T., et al. (2010). A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(3), 256–266. doi:10.1097/00004583-201003000-00009.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Carter, A., Messinger, D., Yirmiya, N., Zwaigenbaum, L., et al. (2011). Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: A baby siblings research consortium study. Pediatrics, 128(3), e488–e495. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-2825.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Parlade, M., Messinger, D. S., Delgado, C. F., Kaiser, M., Van Hecke, A., & Mundy, P. C. (2009). Anticipatory smiling: Linking early affective communication and social outcome. Infant Behavior & Development, 32(1), 33–43. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.09.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Presmanes, A. G., Walden, T. A., Stone, W. L., & Yoder, P. J. (2007). Effects of different attentional cues on responding to joint attention in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 133–144. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0338-0.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc.Google Scholar
  39. Raudenbush, S., Bryk, A., Cheong, Y. F., Congdon, R., & du Toit, M. (2004). HLM 6: Hierarchical linear & nonlinear modeling. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International Inc.Google Scholar
  40. Rozga, A., Hutman, T., Young, G. S., Rogers, S. J., Ozonoff, S., Dapretto, M., et al. (2011). Behavioral profiles of affected and unaffected siblings of children with autism: Contribution of measures of mother–infant interaction and nonverbal communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(3), 287–301. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1051-6.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Tierney, A. L., Gabard-Durnam, L., Vogel-Farley, V., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2012). Developmental trajectories of resting EEG power: An endophenotype of autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One, 7(6), e39127. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039127.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Toth, K., Munson, J., Meltzoff, A. N., & Dawson, G. (2006). Early predictors of communication development in young children with autism spectrum disorder: Joint attention, imitation, and toy play. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(8), 993–1005. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0137-7.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Venezia, M., Messinger, D. S., Thorp, D., & Mundy, P. (2004). The development of anticipatory smiling. Infancy, 6(3), 397–406. doi:10.1207/s15327078in0603_5.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Yirmiya, N., Gamliel, I., Pilowsky, T., Feldman, R., Baron-Cohen, S., & Sigman, M. (2006). The development of siblings of children with autism at 4 and 14 months: Social engagement, communication, and cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(5), 511–523. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01528.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Yoder, P., Stone, W. L., Walden, T., & Malesa, E. (2009). Predicting social impairment and ASD diagnosis in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(10), 1381–1391. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0753.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Young, G. S., Merin, N., Rogers, S. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: Predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. Developmental Science, 12(5), 798–814. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00833.x.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Devon N. Gangi
    • 1
  • Lisa V. Ibañez
    • 2
  • Daniel S. Messinger
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of MiamiCoral GablesUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Washington Autism CenterSeattleUSA
  3. 3.Departments of Psychology, Pediatrics, and Electrical and Computer EngineeringUniversity of MiamiCoral GablesUSA

Personalised recommendations