Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 44, Issue 6, pp 1332–1346 | Cite as

Using the Circumplex Model of Affect to Study Valence and Arousal Ratings of Emotional Faces by Children and Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Angela TsengEmail author
  • Ravi Bansal
  • Jun Liu
  • Andrew J. Gerber
  • Suzanne Goh
  • Jonathan Posner
  • Tiziano Colibazzi
  • Molly Algermissen
  • I-Chin Chiang
  • James A. Russell
  • Bradley S. Peterson
Original Paper

Abstract

The Affective Circumplex Model holds that emotions can be described as linear combinations of two underlying, independent neurophysiological systems (arousal, valence). Given research suggesting individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) have difficulty processing emotions, we used the circumplex model to compare how individuals with ASD and typically-developing (TD) individuals respond to facial emotions. Participants (51 ASD, 80 TD) rated facial expressions along arousal and valence dimensions; we fitted closed, smooth, 2-dimensional curves to their ratings to examine overall circumplex contours. We modeled individual and group influences on parameters describing curve contours to identify differences in dimensional effects across groups. Significant main effects of diagnosis indicated the ASD-group’s ratings were constricted for the entire circumplex, suggesting range constriction across all emotions. Findings did not change when covarying for overall intelligence.

Keywords

Circumplex model of affect Valence Arousal Autism spectrum disorders Facial emotion 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by NIMH grants MH36197, and MHK02-74677, T32-MH16434, T32-MH18264, funding from the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression, and the Suzanne Crosby Murphy Endowment at Columbia University.

Supplementary material

10803_2013_1993_MOESM1_ESM.doc (971 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOC 971 kb)

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, DSM-IV-TR (4th ed.). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing, Incorporated.Google Scholar
  2. Ashwin, C., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., O’Riordan, M., & Bullmore, E. T. (2007). Differential activation of the amygdala and the ‘social brain’ during fearful face-processing in Asperger Syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 2–14.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ashwin, C., Chapman, E., Colle, L., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Impaired recognition of negative basic emotions in autism: a test of the amygdala theory. Social Neuroscience, 1(3–4), 349–363.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bal, E., Harden, E., Lamb, D., Van Hecke, A. V., Denver, J. W., & Porges, S. W. (2010). Emotion recognition in children with autism spectrum disorders: Relations to eye gaze and autonomic state. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(3), 358–370.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barrett, L., & Wager, T. (2006). Structure of emotion: Evidence from neuroimaging studies. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 79–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Batty, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2006). The development of emotional face processing during childhood. Developmental Science, 9(2), 207–220.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Beauchaine, T. P., Neuhaus, E., Zalewski, M., Crowell, S. E., & Potapova, N. (2011). The effects of allostatic load on neural systems subserving motivation, mood regulation, and social affiliation. Development and Psychopathology, 23(4), 975–999.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Ben-Shalom, D., Mostofsky, S. H., Hazlett, R. L., Goldberg, M. C., Landa, R. J., Faran, Y., et al. (2006). Normal physiological emotions but differences in expression of conscious feelings in children with high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(3), 395–400.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Berridge, K. C. (2003). Comparing the emotional brains of humans and other animals. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. Hill Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 25–51). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Blair, R. J. R. (1999). Psychophysiological responsiveness to the distress of others in children with autism. Personality and Individual Differences, 26(3), 477–485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bolte, S., Feineis-Matthews, S., & Poustka, F. (2008). Brief report: Emotional processing in high-functioning autism–physiological reactivity and affective report. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(4), 776–781.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Flanagan, T. D., & Bowler, D. M. (2004). On mosaics and melting pots: Conceptual considerations of comparison and matching strategies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 65–73.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M., & Ito, T. A. (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 173–191). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  14. Capps, L., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. (1992). Understanding of simple and complex emotions in non-retarded children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(7), 1169–1182.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Carey, S. (1992). Becoming a face expert. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences, 335(1273), 95–102.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Castelli, F. (2005). Understanding emotions from standardized facial expressions in autism and normal development. Autism, 9(4), 428–449.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Celani, G., Battacchi, M. W., & Arcidiacono, L. (1999). The understanding of the emotional meaning of facial expressions in people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(1), 57–66.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Colibazzi, T., Posner, J., Wang, Z., Gorman, D., Gerber, A., Yu, S., et al. (2010). Neural systems subserving valence and arousal during the experience of induced emotions. Emotion, 10(3), 377–389.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2005). Social responsiveness scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  20. Davidson, R. J. (2003). Seven sins in the study of emotion: Correctives from affective neuroscience. Brain and Cognition, 52(1), 129–132.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 479–485.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Di Martino, A., Ross, K., Uddin, L. Q., Sklar, A. B., Castellanos, F. X., & Milham, M. P. (2009). Functional brain correlates of social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disorders: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 65(1), 63–74.Google Scholar
  23. Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99(3), 550–553.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, 48(4), 384–392.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of facial affect Palo Alto. CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  26. Feldman-Barrett, L., & Russell, J. A. (1998). Independence and bipolarity in the structure of current affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(4), 967–984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gerber, A. J., Posner, J., Gorman, D., Colibazzi, T., Yu, S., Wang, Z., et al. (2008). An affective circumplex model of neural systems subserving valence, arousal, and cognitive overlay during the appraisal of emotional faces. Neuropsychologia, 46(8), 2129–2139.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Giardina, C. R., & Kuhl, F. P. (1977). Accuracy of curve approximation by harmonically related vectors with elliptical loci. Computer Graphics and Image Processing, 6(3), 277–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hill, J. (2006). The Cambridge Mindreading (CAM) Face-Voice Battery: Testing complex emotion recognition in adults with and without Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(2), 169–183.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gotham, K., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2009). Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(5), 693–705.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gotham, K., Risi, S., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2007). The Autism diagnostic observation schedule: Revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 613–627.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Grelotti, D. J., Gauthier, I., & Schultz, R. T. (2002). Social interest and the development of cortical face specialization: What autism teaches us about face processing. Developmental Psychobiology, 40(3), 213–225.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Harms, M. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology Review, 20(3), 290–322.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hill, E., Berthoz, S., & Frith, U. (2004). Brief report: Cognitive processing of own emotions in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder and in their relatives. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 229–235.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hobson, R. P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  36. Hollingshead, A. (1975). Four-factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Hubert, B. E., Wicker, B., Monfardini, E., & Deruelle, C. (2009). Electrodermal reactivity to emotion processing in adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 13(1), 9–19.Google Scholar
  38. Humphreys, K., Minshew, N., Leonard, G. L., & Behrmann, M. (2007). A fine-grained analysis of facial expression processing in high-functioning adults with autism. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 685–695.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kaartinen, M., Puura, K., Makela, T., Rannisto, M., Lemponen, R., Helminen, M., et al. (2012). Autonomic arousal to direct gaze correlates with social impairments among children with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(9), 1917–1927.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kliemann, D., Dziobek, I., Hatri, A., Steimke, R., & Heekeren, H. R. (2010). Atypical reflexive gaze patterns on emotional faces in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(37), 12281–12287.Google Scholar
  41. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 345–360.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809–816.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Kraemer, H. C., Yesavage, J. A., Taylor, J. L., & Kupfer, D. (2000). How can we learn about developmental processes from cross-sectional studies, or can we? American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(2), 163–171.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kring, A. M., Feldman-Barrett, L., & Gard, D. E. (2003). On the broad applicability of the affective circumplex. Psychological Science, 14(3), 207–214.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Kuhl, F. P., & Giardina, C. R. (1982). Elliptic fourier features of a closed contour. Computer Graphics and Image Processing, 18(3), 236–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Kuhn, G., Benson, V., Fletcher-Watson, S., Kovshoff, H., McCormick, C. A., Kirkby, J., et al. (2010). Eye movements affirm: Automatic overt gaze and arrow cueing for typical adults and adults with autism spectrum disorder. Experimental Brain Research, 201(2), 155–165.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Kylliäinen, A., & Hietanen, J. K. (2006). Skin conductance responses to another person’s gaze in children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(4), 517–525.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1999). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings Gainesville: University of Florida, Center for Research in Psychophysiology.Google Scholar
  49. Legiša, J., Messinger, D., Kermol, E., & Marlier, L. (2013). Emotional responses to odors in children with high-functioning autism: Autonomic arousal, facial behavior and self-report. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(4), 869–879.Google Scholar
  50. Lioy, D. T., Wu, W. W., & Bissonnette, J. M. (2011). Autonomic dysfunction with mutations in the gene that encodes methyl-CpG-binding protein 2: Insights into Rett syndrome. Autonomic Neuroscience, 161(1–2), 55–62.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., et al. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: a standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(2), 185–212.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Losh, M., & Capps, L. (2006). Understanding of emotional experience in autism: Insights from the personal accounts of high-functioning children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 42(5), 809–818.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Martineau, J., Hernandez, N., Hiebel, L., Roche, L., Metzger, A., & Bonnet-Brilhault, F. (2011). Can pupil size and pupil responses during visual scanning contribute to the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in children? Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(8), 1077–1082.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Ming, X., Bain, J. M., Smith, D., Brimacombe, M., Gold von-Simson, G., & Axelrod, F. B. (2011). Assessing autonomic dysfunction symptoms in children: A pilot study. Journal of Child Neurology, 26(4), 420–427.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Ming, X., Julu, P. O., Brimacombe, M., Connor, S., & Daniels, M. L. (2005). Reduced cardiac parasympathetic activity in children with autism. Brain and Development, 27(7), 509–516.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Ortony, A., & Turner, T. J. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions? Psychological Review, 97(3), 315–331.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Osterling, J. A., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A. (2002). Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology, 14(2), 239–251.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1990). Are there emotion perception deficits in young autistic children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31(3), 343–361.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Panksepp, J. (1992). A critical role for “affective neuroscience” in resolving what is basic about basic emotions. Psychological Review, 99(3), 554–560.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Parish-Morris, J., Chevallier, C., Tonge, N., Letzen, J., Pandey, J., & Schultz, R. T. (2013). Visual attention to dynamic faces and objects is linked to face processing skills: A combined study of children with autism and controls. Front Psychol, 4, 185.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Patriquin, M. A., Scarpa, A., Friedman, B. H., & Porges, S. W. (2013). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: A marker for positive social functioning and receptive language skills in children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(2), 101–112.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(4), 249–261.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1992). The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evidence from normal infants and children with autism or mental handicap. Development and Psychopathology, 4(03), 375–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Piepers, D. W., & Robbins, R. A. (2012). A review and clarification of the terms “holistic,” “configural,” and “relational” in the face perception literature. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 559.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 116–143.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., Portales, A. L., & Greenspan, S. I. (1996). Infant regulation of the vagal “brake” predicts child behavior problems: A psychobiological model of social behavior. Developmental Psychobiology, 29(8), 697–712.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Posner, J., Russell, J. A., Gerber, A., Gorman, D., Colibazzi, T., Yu, S., et al. (2009). The neurophysiological bases of emotion: An fMRI study of the affective circumplex using emotion-denoting words. Human Brain Mapping, 30(3), 883–895.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Posner, J., Russell, J. A., & Peterson, B. S. (2005). The circumplex model of affect: an integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 17(3), 715–734.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Riby, D. M., Doherty-Sneddon, G., & Bruce, V. (2008). Exploring face perception in disorders of development: evidence from Williams syndrome and autism. Journal of Neuropsychology, 2(Pt 1), 47–64.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Rieffe, C., Meerum Terwogt, M., & Kotronopoulou, K. (2007). Awareness of single and multiple emotions in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(3), 455–465.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Russell, J. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161–1178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Russell, J. A. (2005). Emotion in human consciousness is built on core affect. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 12, 26–42.Google Scholar
  75. Russell, J., & Bullock, M. (1985). Multidimensional scaling of emotional facial expressions: Similarity from preschoolers to adults. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1290–1298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Russell, J., Weiss, A., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1989). Affect grid: A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 493–502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Rutherford, M. D., & McIntosh, D. N. (2007). Rules versus prototype matching: Strategies of perception of emotional facial expressions in the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(2), 187–196.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Schlosberg, H. (1952). The description of facial expressions in terms of two dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44(4), 229–237.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., et al. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and nonautistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(5), 747–753.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Tantam, D., Monaghan, L., Nicholson, H., & Stirling, J. (1989). Autistic children’s ability to interpret faces: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30(4), 623–630.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Vaughan Van Hecke, A., Lebow, J., Bal, E., Lamb, D., Harden, E., Kramer, A., et al. (2009). Electroencephalogram and heart rate regulation to familiar and unfamiliar people in children with autism spectrum disorders. Child Development, 80(4), 1118–1133.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Wallace, G. L., Case, L. K., Harms, M. B., Silvers, J. A., Kenworthy, L., & Martin, A. (2011). Diminished sensitivity to sad facial expressions in high functioning autism spectrum disorders is associated with symptomatology and adaptive functioning. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(11), 1475–1486.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98(2), 219–235.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI) San Antonio. TX: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
  85. Wright, B., Clarke, N., Jordan, J., Young, A. W., Clarke, P., Miles, J., et al. (2008). Emotion recognition in faces and the use of visual context in young people with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 12(6), 607–626.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Angela Tseng
    • 1
    Email author
  • Ravi Bansal
    • 1
  • Jun Liu
    • 1
  • Andrew J. Gerber
    • 1
  • Suzanne Goh
    • 1
  • Jonathan Posner
    • 1
  • Tiziano Colibazzi
    • 1
  • Molly Algermissen
    • 1
  • I-Chin Chiang
    • 1
  • James A. Russell
    • 2
  • Bradley S. Peterson
    • 1
  1. 1.Child and Adolescent PsychiatryColumbia University College of Physicians & SurgeonsNew YorkUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyBoston CollegeBostonUSA

Personalised recommendations