Skip to main content
Log in

The Mechanisms Underlying the ASD Advantage in Visual Search

  • Perception In Autism
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A number of studies have demonstrated that individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are faster or more successful than typically developing control participants at various visual-attentional tasks (for reviews, see Dakin and Frith in Neuron 48:497–507, 2005; Simmons et al. in Vis Res 49:2705–2739, 2009). This “ASD advantage” was first identified in the domain of visual search by Plaisted et al. (J Child Psychol Psychiatry 39:777–783, 1998). Here we survey the findings of visual search studies from the past 15 years that contrasted the performance of individuals with and without ASD. Although there are some minor caveats, the overall consensus is that—across development and a broad range of symptom severity—individuals with ASD reliably outperform controls on visual search. The etiology of the ASD advantage has not been formally specified, but has been commonly attributed to ‘enhanced perceptual discrimination’, a superior ability to visually discriminate between targets and distractors in such tasks (e.g. O’Riordan in Cognition 77:81–96, 2000). As well, there is considerable evidence for impairments of the attentional network in ASD (for a review, see Keehn et al. in J Child Psychol Psychiatry 37:164–183, 2013). We discuss some recent results from our laboratory that support an attentional, rather than perceptual explanation for the ASD advantage in visual search. We speculate that this new conceptualization may offer a better understanding of some of the behavioral symptoms associated with ASD, such as over-focusing and restricted interests.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It is important to note that an opposite trend, namely increased distractibility has also been reported in ASD. Burack (1994) tested a small group of extremely low-functioning (N = 12, mean IQ = 49.5) adults with ASD and suggested that there is a general selective attention deficit in autism. Many authors since this early report have demonstrated (e.g. Remington et al. 2009) evidence for the contrary. A general selective attention deficit is also in contrast with the findings of the large body of research on ASD advantage in visual search reviewed in this paper.

References

  • Almeida, R. A., Dickinson, J., Maybery, M. T., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2010). Visual search performance in the autism spectrum II: The radial frequency search task with additional segmentation cues. Neuropsychologia, 48, 4117–4124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Almeida, R. A., Dickinson, J., Maybery, M. T., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2013). Visual search targeting either local or global perceptual processes differs as a function of autistic-like traits in the typically developing population. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1272–1286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. J., & Colombo, J. (2009). Larger tonic pupil size in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Psychobiology, 51, 207–211.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. J., Colombo, J., & Shaddy, D. (2006). Visual scanning and pupillary responses in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 1238–1256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. J., Colombo, J., & Unruh, K. E. (2012). Pupil and salivary indicators of autonomic dysfunction in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Psychobiology,. doi:10.1002/dev.21051.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ashwin, C., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Finding a face in the crowd: Testing the anger superiority effect in Asperger Syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 61, 78–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). An Integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 403–450.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aston-Jones, G., Iba, M., Clayton, E., Rajkowski, J., & Cohen, J. (2007). The locus coeruleus and regulation of behavioral flexibility and attention: Clinical implications. In G. A. Ordway, M. A. Schwartz, & A. Frazer (Eds.), Brain norepinephrine: Neurobiology and therapeutics (pp. 196–235). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baldassi, S., Pei, F., Megna, N., Recupero, G., Viespoli, M., Igliozzi, R., et al. (2009). Search superiority in autism within, but not outside the crowding regime. Vision Research, 49, 2151–2156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baranek, G. T. (1999). Autism during infancy: A retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor and social behaviors at 9–12 months of age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 213–224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayliss, A. P., & Kritikos, A. (2011). Brief report: Perceptual load and the autism spectrum in typically developed individuals. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1573–1578.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beatty, J. (1982). Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 276–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blaser, E., Eglington, L., & Kaldy, Z. (2012). Toddlers with ASD are better at visual search without trying harder: A pupillometric study. Poster presented at the Annual Meeting of the Vision Sciences Society, May 11-16, 2012, Naples, FL.

  • Blaser, E., Eglington, L., Carter, A. S., & Kaldy, Z. (2013). Pupillometry reveals a mechanism for the Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) advantage in visual tasks (submitted).

  • Bölte, S., Schlitt, S., Gapp, V., Hainz, D., Schirman, S., Poustka, F., et al. (2012). A close eye on the eagle-eyed visual acuity hypothesis of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 726–733.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, J., Xu, J. Y., & Brooks, K. R. (2011). Individual differences in visual search: Relationship to autistic traits, discrimination thresholds, and speed of processing. Perception, 40, 739–742.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruckner, C., & Yoder, P. (2007). Restricted object use in young children with autism: Definition and construct validity. Autism, 11, 161–171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burack, J. A. (1994). Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: Preliminary evidence of an inefficient attentional lens. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 535–543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caron, M. J., Mottron, L. L., Berthiaume, C. C., & Dawson, M. M. (2006). Cognitive mechanisms, specificity and neural underpinnings of visuospatial peaks in autism. Brain, 129, 1789–1802.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collignon, O., Charbonneau, G., Peters, F., Nassim, M., Lassonde, M., Lepore, F., et al. (2013). Reduced multisensory facilitation in persons with autism. Cortex, 49, 1704–1710.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, J. (2012, November 29). The autism advantage. The New York Times Magazine. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com.

  • Dakin, S., & Frith, U. (2005). Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Neuron, 48, 497–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darling, R. D., Alzghoul, L., Zhang, J., Khatri, N., Paul, I. A., Simpson, K. L., et al. (2011). Perinatal citalopram exposure selectively increases locus coeruleus circuit function in male rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 16709–16715.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G. (2008). Early behavioral intervention, brain plasticity, and the prevention of autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 775–803.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., Carver, L., & McPartland, J. (2001). Brief report: recognition memory and stimulus-reward associations: indirect support for the role of ventromedial prefrontal dysfunction in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 337–341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsabbagh, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2007). Infancy and autism: progress, prospects, and challenges. Progress in Brain Research, 164, 355–383.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsabbagh, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2010). Getting answers from babies about autism. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 14, 81–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsabbagh, M., Volein, A., Holmboe, K., Tucker, L., Csibra, G., Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (2009). Visual orienting in the early broader autism phenotype: disengagement and facilitation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 637–642.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Falkmer, M., Stuart, G. W., Danielsson, H., Bram, S., Lönebrink, M., & Falkmer, T. (2011). Visual acuity in adults with Asperger’s syndrome: No evidence for “eagle-eyed” vision. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 812–816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, A., Sowden, P., Notman, L., Gonzalez-Dixon, M., West, D., Alexander, I., et al. (2010). Reduced chromatic discrimination in children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Science, 13, 188–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C., Ehlers, S., Schaumann, H., & Jakobsson, G. (1990). Autism under age 3 years: A clinical study of 28 cases referred for autistic symptoms in infancy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 921–934.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harms, M. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology Review, 20, 290–322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollander, E., Kaplan, A., Cartwright, C., & Reichman, D. (2000). Venlafaxine in children, adolescents, and young adults with autism spectrum disorders: An open retrospective clinical report. Journal of Child Neurology, 15, 132–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iarocci, G., Burack, J. A., Shore, D. I., Mottron, L., & Enns, J. T. (2006). Global-local visual processing in high functioning children with autism: Structural vs. implicit task biases. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 117–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, I., & Sirois, S. (2009). Infant cognition: going full factorial with pupil dilation. Developmental Science, 12, 670–679.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C., Gilchrist, I. D., & Bender, A. (2005). Embedded figures detection in autism and typical development: preliminary evidence of a double dissociation in relationships with visual search. Developmental Science, 8, 344–351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, R. M., Keehn, B., Connolly, C., Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2009). Why is visual search superior in autism spectrum disorder? Developmental Science, 12, 1083–1096.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Julesz, B. (1981). Textons, the elements of texture perception, and their interactions. Nature, 290, 91–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Beatty, J. (1966). Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science, 154, 1583–1585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaldy, Z., Kraper, C., Carter, A. S., & Blaser, E. (2011). Toddlers with autism spectrum disorder are more successful at visual search than typically developing toddlers. Developmental Science, 14, 980–988.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kawakubo, Y., Kasai, K., Okazaki, S., Hosokawa-Kakurai, M., Watanabe, K., Kuwabara, H., et al. (2007). Electrophysiological abnormalities of spatial attention in adults with autism during the gap overlap task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 1464–1471.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keehn, B., & Joseph, R. M. (2008). Impaired prioritization of novel onset stimuli in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 1296–1303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keehn, B., Müller, R. A., & Townsend, J. (2013). Atypical attentional networks and the emergence of autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 164–183.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Koh, H. C., Milne, E., & Dobkins, K. (2010). Spatial contrast sensitivity in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 978–987.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laeng, B., Ørbo, M., Holmlund, T., & Miozzo, M. (2011). Pupillary Stroop effects. Cognitive Processing, 12, 13–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1115–1122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liss, M., Saulnier, C., Fein, D., & Kinsbourne, M. (2006). Sensory and attention abnormalities in autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 10, 155–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mangelsdorf, S. C., Shapiro, J. R., & Marzolf, D. (1995). Developmental and temperamental differences in emotional regulation in infancy. Child Development, 66, 1817–1828.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mehler, M. F., & Purpura, D. P. (2009). Autism, fever, epigenetics and the locus coeruleus. Brain Research Reviews, 59, 388–392.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., Dunn, S. A., Freeth, M., & Rosas-Martinez, L. (2013). Visual search performance is predicted by the degree to which selective attention to features modulates the ERP between 350 and 600 ms. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1109–1118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minzenberg, M. J., Watrous, A. J., Yoon, J. H., Ursu, S., & Carter, C. S. (2008). Modafinil shifts human locus coeruleus to low-tonic, high-phasic activity during functional MRI. Science, 322, 1700–1702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L. (2011). Changing perceptions: The power of autism. Nature, 479, 33–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Bouvet, L., Bonnel, A., Samson, F., Burack, J. A., Dawson, M., et al. (2013). Veridical mapping in the development of exceptional autistic abilities. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 209–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., & Burack, J. A. (2001). Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 131–148). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 27–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Mineau, S., Bernier, C. S., Berthiaume, C., Dawson, M., Lemay, M., et al. (2007). Lateral glances toward moving stimuli among young children with autism: Early regulation of locally oriented perception? Development and Psychopathology, 19, 23–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nassar, M. R., Rumsey, K. M., Wilson, R. C., Parikh, K., Heasly, B., & Gold, J. I. (2012). Rational regulation of learning dynamics by pupil-linked arousal systems. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 1040–1046.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M. (2000). Superior modulation of activation levels of stimulus representations does not underlie superior discrimination in autism. Cognition, 77, 81–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M. A. (2004). Superior visual search in adults with autism. Autism, 8, 229–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M., & Plaisted, K. (2001). Enhanced discrimination in autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 54A, 961–979.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 719–730.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Macari, S., Young, G. S., Goldring, S., Thompson, M., & Rogers, S. J. (2008). Atypical object exploration at 12 months of age is associated with autism in a prospective sample. Autism, 12, 457–472.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73–89.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 777–783.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, G., Troscianko, T., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2007). Effort during visual search and counting: Insights from pupillometry. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 211–229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Remington, A., Swettenham, J., Campbell, R., & Coleman, M. (2009). Selective attention and perceptual load in autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Science, 20, 1388–1393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Remington, A. M., Swettenham, J. G., & Lavie, N. (2012). Lightening the load: Perceptual load impairs visual detection in typical adults but not in autism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 544–551.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Riby, D. M., Brown, P. H., Jones, N., & Hanley, M. (2012). Brief report: Faces cause less distraction in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 634–639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 763–781.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sara, S. J. (2009). The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 211–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saslow, M. G. (1967). Effects of components of displacement step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. Journal of The Optical Society Of America, 57, 1024–1029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sasson, N., Elison, J., Turner-Brown, L., Dichter, G., & Bodfish, J. (2011). Brief report: Circumscribed attention in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 242–247.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Holtzclaw, T. N., Lam, K. S. L., & Bodfish, J. W. (2008). Children with autism demonstrate circumscribed attention during passive viewing of complex social and nonsocial picture arrays. Autism Research, 1, 31–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, A. (1988). Visuospatial islets of ability and intellectual functioning in autism. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of London.

  • Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in Autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 4, 613–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1351–1364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, D. R., Robertson, A. E., McKay, L. S., Toal, E., McAleer, P., & Pollick, F. E. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 49, 2705–2739.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Camfferman, G., Verbaten, M. N., & Van Engeland, H. (2001). Eye movements, visual attention, and autism: A saccadic reaction time study using the gap and overlap paradigm. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 614–619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A. M., Woods, J., Allen, L., Cleary, J., Dickinson, H., & Lord, C. (2004). Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 473–493.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1998). What can 1,000,000 trials tell us about visual search? Psychological Science, 9, 33–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2004). What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Lord, C., Rogers, S., Carter, A., Carver, L., et al. (2009). Clinical assessment and management of toddlers with suspected autism spectrum disorder: Insights from studies of high-risk infants. Pediatrics, 123, 1383–1391.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 143–152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant 2R15EY017985 awarded to EB and ZK and by the University of Massachusetts Presidential Science and Technology Award awarded to ZK, EB and ASC.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Zsuzsa Kaldy.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kaldy, Z., Giserman, I., Carter, A.S. et al. The Mechanisms Underlying the ASD Advantage in Visual Search. J Autism Dev Disord 46, 1513–1527 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1957-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1957-x

Keywords

Navigation