Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 44, Issue 3, pp 584–592 | Cite as

Visual Attention to Competing Social and Object Images by Preschool Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Original Paper

Abstract

Eye tracking studies of young children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) report a reduction in social attention and an increase in visual attention to non-social stimuli, including objects related to circumscribed interests (CI) (e.g., trains). In the current study, fifteen preschoolers with ASD and 15 typically developing controls matched on gender and age (range 24–62 months) were eye tracked while viewing a paired preference task of face and object stimuli. While co-varying verbal and nonverbal developmental quotients, preschoolers with ASD were similar to controls in their visual attention to faces presented with objects unrelated to CI, but attended significantly less than controls to faces presented with CI-related objects. This was consistent across three metrics: preference, prioritization and duration. Social attention in preschoolers with ASD therefore appears modulated by salience of competing non-social stimuli, which may affect the development of both social and non-social characteristics of the disorder.

Keywords

Autism Social attention Eye-tracking Face processing Circumscribed interests Restricted interests 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Thomas Campbell and Christine Dollaghan for generously sharing their lab space, and Suzanne Bonifert, Jamie Cato, Hillary Carrington and Lark Huang-Storms for their assistance with recruitment and clinical evaluations. We also extend our gratitude to all participating children and their families. This research was supported by Grant Number UL1RR024982, titled, “North and Central Texas Clinical and Translational Science Initiative” (Milton Packer, M.D., PI) from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), a component of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) and NIH Roadmap for Medical Research, and its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official view of the NCRR or NIH. Information on NCRR is available at http://www.ncrr.nih.gov/. Information on Re-engineering the Clinical Research Enterprise can be obtained from http://nihroadmap.nih.gov/clinicalresearch/overview-translational.asp.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Attwood, T. (1998). Asperger’s syndrome: A guide for parents and professionals. Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  2. Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., & Hackett, G. (2009). Fetal testosterone and autistic traits. British Journal of Psychology, 100, 1–22.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bhat, A. N., Galloway, J. C., & Landa, R. J. (2010). Social and non-social visual attention patterns and associative learning in infants at risk for autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 989–997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bryson, S. E., Zwaigenbaum, L., Brian, J., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Rombough, V., et al. (2007). A prospective case series of high-risk infants who developed autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 12–24.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2012). Context modulates attention to social scenes in toddlers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 903–913.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2013). Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 74, 195–203.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 231–239.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dawson, G., Carver, L., Meltzoff, A. N., Panagiotides, H., McPartland, J., & Webb, S. J. (2002). Neural correlates of face and object recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development. Child Development, 73, 700–717.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. DeLoache, J. S., Simcock, G., & Macari, S. (2007). Planes, trains, automobiles—And tea sets: Extremely intense interests in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1579–1586.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dichter, G. S., Felder, J. N., Green, S. R., Rittenberg, A. M., Sasson, N. J., & Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Reward circuitry function in autism spectrum disorders. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 160–172.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Elison, J. T., Paterson, S. J., Wolff, J. J., Reznick, J. S., Sasson, N. J., Gu, H., et al. (2013). White matter microstructure and atypical visual orienting in 7 month-olds at risk for autism. American Journal of Psychiatry. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12091150.
  12. Elison, J. T., Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Dichter, G. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Age trends in visual exploration of social and nonsocial information in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 842–851.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Elsabbagh, M., Fernandes, J., Webb, S. J., Dawson, G., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H., et al. (2013). Disengagement of visual attention in infancy is associated with emerging autism in toddlerhood. Biological Psychiatry, 74, 189–194.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Elsabbagh, M., Holmboe, K., Gliga, T., Mercure, E., Hudry, K., Charman, T., et al. (2011). Social and attention factors during infancy and the later emergence of autism characteristics. Progress in Brain Research, 189, 195–207.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Elsabbagh, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2010). Getting answers from babies about autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 81–87.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Esbensen, A. J., Seltzer, M. M., Lam, K. S. L., & Bodfish, J. W. (2009). Age-related differences in restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 57–66.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., & Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and brain development. Child Development, 58, 539–559.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Grelotti, D. J., Gauthier, I., & Schultz, R. T. (2002). Social interest and the development of cortical face specialization: What autism teaches us about face processing. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 213–225.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gur, R., Schroeder, L., Turner, T., McGrath, C., Chan, R., Turetsky, B., et al. (2002). Brain activation during facial emotion processing. NeuroImage, 16, 651.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hollander, E., Novotny, S., Hanratty, M., Yaffe, R., DeCaria, C. M., Aronowitz, B. R., et al. (2003). Oxytocin infusion reduces repetitive behavior in adults with autistic and Asperger’s disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 28, 193–198.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hutman, T., Chela, M. K., Gillespie-Lynch, K., & Sigman, M. (2012). Selective visual attention at twelve months: Signs of autism in early social interactions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 487–498.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Johnson, M. H. (2000). Cortical specialization for higher cognitive functions: Beyond the maturational model. Brain and Cognition, 42, 124–127.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Johnson, M., Griffin, R., Csibra, G., Halit, H., Farronni, T., de Haan, M., et al. (2005). The emergence of the social brain network: Evidence from typical and atypical development. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 599–619.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(8), 946–954.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Jordan, C. J., & Caldwell-Harris, C. L. (2012). Understanding differences in neurotypical and autism spectrum special interests through internet forums. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 50, 391–402.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Klin, A., Danovitch, J. H., Merz, A. B., & Volkmar, F. (2007). Circumscribed interests in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: An exploratory study. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 32, 89–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R. T., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. J. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, 459, 257–261.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Koegel, R. L., Firestone, P. B., Kramme, K. W., & Dunlap, G. (1974). Increasing spontaneous play by suppressing self-stimulation in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 521–528.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kohler, C. G., Turner, T. H., Bilker, W. B., Brensinger, C. M., Siegel, S. J., Kanes, S. J., et al. (2003). Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: Intensity effects and error pattern. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1768–1774.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Koterba, E. A., Leezenbaum, N. B., & Iverson, J. M. (2012). Object exploration at 6 and 9 months in infants with and without risk for autism. Autism. doi: 10.1177/1362361312464826.
  32. Lam, K. S., Bodfish, J. W., & Piven, J. (2008). Evidence for three subtypes of repetitive behaviors in autism that differ in familiality and association with other symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 1193–1200.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H, Jr, Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Morgan, L., Wetherby, A. M., & Barber, A. (2008). Repetitive and stereotyped movements in children with autism spectrum disorders late in the second year of life. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 826–837.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning (AGS Edition ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc.Google Scholar
  36. Nadig, A., Lee, I., Singh, L., Bosshart, K., & Ozonoff, S. (2010). How does the topic of conversation affect verbal exchange and eye gaze? A comparison between typical development and high-functioning autism. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2730–2739.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Osterling, J. A., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A. (2002). Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology, 14(2), 239–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ozonoff, S., Macari, S., Young, G. S., Goldring, S., Thompson, M., & Rogers, S. J. (2008). Atypical object exploration at 12 months of age is associated with autism in a prospective sample. Autism, 12, 457–472.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Parish-Morris, J., Chevallier, C., Tonge, N., Letzen, J., Pandey, J., & Schultz, R. T. (2013). Visual attention to dynamic faces and objects is linked to face processing skills: A combined study of children with autism and controls. Frontiers in Developmental Psychology, 4(185). doi:  10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00185.
  40. Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. N., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 249–261.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Pierce, K., Conant, D., Hazin, R., Stoner, R., & Desmond, J. (2011). Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 101–109.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sasson, N. J. (2006). The Development of Face Processing in Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 381–394.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Sasson, N. J., Dichter, G. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Affective responses by adults with autism are reduced to social images but elevated to images related to circumscribed interests. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e42457. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042457.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sasson, N. J., Elison, J. T., Turner-Brown, L. M., Dichter, G. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2011). Brief report: Circumscribed attention in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 242–247.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Sasson, N. J., Pinkham, A. E., Richard, J., Hughett, P., Gur, R. E., & Gur, R. C. (2010). Controlling for response biases clarifies sex and age differences in facial affect recognition. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34, 207–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Sasson, N., Tsuchiya, N., Hurley, R., Couture, S. M., Penn, D. L., Adolphs, R., et al. (2007). Orienting to social stimuli differentiates social cognitive impairment in autism and schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2580–2588.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Holtzclaw, T. N., Lam, K. S. L., & Bodfish, J. W. (2008). Children with autism demonstrate circumscribed attention during passive viewing of complex social and nonsocial picture arrays. Autism Research, 1, 31–42.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Schultz, R. T. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 125–141.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. South, M., Ozonoff, S., & McMahon, W. M. (2005). Repetitive behavior profiles in Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 145–158.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Turner-Brown, L. M. T., Lam, K. S. L., Holtzclaw, T. N., Dichter, G. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2011). Phenomenology and measurement of circumscribed interests in autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 15, 437–456.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Varni, J. W., Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R. L., & Everett, N. L. (1979). An analysis of observational learning in autistic and normal children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 7, 31–43.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Watt, N., Wetherby, A. M., Barber, A., & Morgan, L. (2008). Repetitive and stereotyped behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1518–1533.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N. (2000). Brief report: Recognition of autism spectrum disorder before one year of age: A retrospective study based on home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(2), 157–162.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 143–152.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences—GR41The University of Texas at DallasRichardsonUSA

Personalised recommendations